Template:Short description {{#invoke:other uses|otheruses}}

Template:Discrimination sidebar Bias is a disproportionate weight in favor of or against an idea or thing, usually in a way that is inaccurate, closed-minded, prejudicial, or unfair. Biases can be innate or learned. People may develop biases for or against an individual, a group, or a belief.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> In science and engineering, a bias is a systematic error. Statistical bias results from an unfair sampling of a population, or from an estimation process that does not give accurate results on average.<ref name="ReferenceA">Template:Cite journal</ref>

EtymologyEdit

The word appears to derive from Old Provençal into Old French biais, "sideways, askance, against the grain". Whence comes French biais, "a slant, a slope, an oblique".<ref name=etymology>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref>

It seems to have entered English via the game of bowls, where it referred to balls made with a greater weight on one side. Which expanded to the figurative use, "a one-sided tendency of the mind", and, at first especially in law, "undue propensity or prejudice".<ref name=etymology /> or ballast, used to lower the centre of gravity of a ship to increase stability and to keep the ship from one side.<ref>Template:Cite book</ref>

TypesEdit

Cognitive biasesEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} A cognitive bias is a repeating or basic misstep in thinking, assessing, recollecting, or other cognitive processes.<ref name=Chegg>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref> That is, a pattern of deviation from standards in judgment, whereby inferences may be created unreasonably.<ref>Template:Cite book</ref> People create their own "subjective social reality" from their own perceptions,<ref>Template:Cite book</ref> their view of the world may dictate their behaviour.<ref>Template:Cite book</ref> Thus, cognitive biases may sometimes lead to perceptual distortion, inaccurate judgment, illogical interpretation, or what is broadly called irrationality.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>Baron, J. (2007). Thinking and Deciding (4th ed.). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.</ref><ref>Ariely, D. (2008). Predictably Irrational: The Hidden Forces That Shape Our Decisions. New York, NY: HarperCollins.</ref> However some cognitive biases are taken to be adaptive, and thus may lead to success in the appropriate situation.<ref>For instance: Template:Cite journal</ref> Furthermore, cognitive biases as an example through education may allow faster choice selection when speedier outcomes for a task are more valuable than precision.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> Other cognitive biases are a "by-product" of human processing limitations,<ref name="Haselton, M. G., Nettle, D., & Andrews, P. W. 2005 724–746">Template:Cite book</ref> coming about because of an absence of appropriate mental mechanisms, or just from human limitations in information processing.<ref>Template:Cite book</ref>

AnchoringEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Anchoring is a psychological heuristic that describes the propensity to rely on the first piece of information encountered when making decisions.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="ScienceDaily">{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref><ref name="Investopedia">{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref> According to this heuristic, individuals begin with an implicitly suggested reference point (the "anchor") and make adjustments to it to reach their estimate.<ref name="ReferenceA"/> For example, the initial price offered for a used car sets the standard for the rest of the negotiations, so that prices lower than the initial price seem more reasonable even if they are still higher than what the car is worth.<ref name="TverskyKahneman1974">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>Edward Teach, "Avoiding Decision Traps Template:Webarchive", CFO (1 June 2004). Retrieved 29 May 2007.</ref>

ApopheniaEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}}Template:See also Apophenia, also known as patternicity,<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>Template:Cite news</ref> or agenticity,<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref> is the human tendency to perceive meaningful patterns within random data. Apophenia is well documented as a rationalization for gambling. Gamblers may imagine that they see patterns in the numbers which appear in lotteries, card games, or roulette wheels.<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref> One manifestation of this is known as the "gambler's fallacy".

Pareidolia is the visual or auditory form of apophenia. It has been suggested that pareidolia combined with hierophany may have helped ancient societies organize chaos and make the world intelligible.<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref><ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref>

Attribution biasEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} An attribution bias can happen when individuals assess or attempt to discover explanations behind their own and others' behaviors.<ref name="Heider">Heider, F. (1958). "The psychology of interpersonal relations", New York: Wiley, 322 p.</ref><ref name="Kelley 1967">Kelley, H.H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. Levine (Ed.) Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> People make attributions about the causes of their own and others' behaviors; but these attributions do not necessarily precisely reflect reality. Rather than operating as objective perceivers, individuals are inclined to perceptual slips that prompt biased understandings of their social world.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="Nisbett and Ross 1980">Nisbett, R.E. & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.</ref> When judging others we tend to assume their actions are the result of internal factors such as personality, whereas we tend to assume our own actions arise because of the necessity of external circumstances. There are a wide range of sorts of attribution biases, such as the ultimate attribution error, fundamental attribution error, actor-observer bias, and self-serving bias.

Examples of attribution bias:<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref>

Confirmation biasEdit

File:Fred Barnard07.jpg
Confirmation bias has been described as an internal "yes man", echoing back a person's beliefs like Charles Dickens' character Uriah Heep.<ref name="WSJ">Template:Cite news</ref>

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}}

Confirmation bias is the tendency to search for, interpret, favor, and recall information in a way that confirms one's beliefs or hypotheses while giving disproportionately less attention to information that contradicts it.<ref>Template:Cite book</ref> The effect is stronger for emotionally charged issues and for deeply entrenched beliefs. People also tend to interpret ambiguous evidence as supporting their existing position. Biased search, interpretation and memory have been invoked to explain attitude polarization (when a disagreement becomes more extreme even though the different parties are exposed to the same evidence), belief perseverance (when beliefs persist after the evidence for them is shown to be false), the irrational primacy effect (a greater reliance on information encountered early in a series) and illusory correlation (when people falsely perceive an association between two events or situations). Confirmation biases contribute to overconfidence in personal beliefs and can maintain or strengthen beliefs in the face of contrary evidence. Poor decisions due to these biases have been found in political and organizational contexts.<ref name=cbiaspolitics>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>Tuchman, Barbara (1984). The March of Folly: From Troy to Vietnam. New York: Knopf.</ref>

FramingEdit

Template:Anchor {{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Framing involves the social construction of social phenomena by mass media sources, political or social movements, political leaders, and so on. It is an influence over how people organize, perceive, and communicate about reality.<ref name="Druckman2001">Template:Cite journal</ref> It can be positive or negative, depending on the audience and what kind of information is being presented. For political purposes, framing often presents facts in such a way that implicates a problem that is in need of a solution. Members of political parties attempt to frame issues in a way that makes a solution favoring their own political leaning appear as the most appropriate course of action for the situation at hand.<ref name="van der Pas">Template:Cite journal</ref> As understood in social theory, framing is a schema of interpretation, a collection of anecdotes and stereotypes, that individuals rely on to understand and respond to events.<ref name="Goffman1974">Goffman, E. (1974). Frame Analysis: An Essay on the Organization of Experience. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.</ref> People use filters to make sense of the world, the choices they then make are influenced by their creation of a frame.

Cultural bias is the related phenomenon of interpreting and judging phenomena by standards inherent to one's own culture. Numerous such biases exist, concerning cultural norms for color, location of body parts, mate selection, concepts of justice, linguistic and logical validity, acceptability of evidence, and taboos. Ordinary people may tend to imagine other people as basically the same, not significantly more or less valuable, probably attached emotionally to different groups and different land.

Halo effect and horn effectEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} The halo effect and the horn effect are when an observer's overall impression of a person, organization, brand, or product influences their feelings about specifics of that entity's character or properties.<ref name="study.com">{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref><ref name="Investopedia halo">{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>

The name halo effect is based on the concept of the saint's halo, and is a specific type of confirmation bias, wherein positive sentiments in one area cause questionable or unknown characteristics to be seen positively. If the observer likes one aspect of something, they will have a positive predisposition toward everything about it.<ref>Template:Cite dictionary</ref><ref name=nisbett>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>Template:Cite news</ref><ref name="ostrove_sigall1975">Template:Cite journal</ref> A person's appearance has been found to produce a halo effect.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> The halo effect is also present in the field of brand marketing, affecting perception of companies and non-governmental organizations (NGOs).<ref>Template:Cite news</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>Template:Cite news</ref><ref>Template:Cite news</ref><ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>

The opposite of the halo is the horn effect, when "individuals believe (that negative) traits are inter-connected."<ref name="JoshKennon">{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref> The term horn effect refers to Devil's horns.Template:Citation needed It works in a negative direction: if the observer dislikes one aspect of something, they will have a negative predisposition towards other aspects.<ref name="Attitudes">{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref>

Self-serving biasEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Self-serving bias is the tendency for cognitive or perceptual processes to be distorted by the individual's need to maintain and enhance self-esteem.<ref>Myers, D.G. (2015). Exploring Social Psychology, 7th Edition. New York: McGraw Hill Education.</ref> It is the propensity to credit accomplishment to our own capacities and endeavors, yet attribute failure to outside factors,<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> to dismiss the legitimacy of negative criticism, concentrate on positive qualities and accomplishments yet disregard flaws and failures. Studies have demonstrated that this bias can affect behavior in the workplace,<ref name=Pal>Template:Cite journal</ref> in interpersonal relationships,<ref name="Campbell et al.">Template:Cite journal</ref> playing sports,<ref name=DeMichele>Template:Cite journal</ref> and in consumer decisions.<ref name=Moon>Template:Cite journal</ref>

Status quo biasEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Status quo bias is an emotional bias; a preference for the current state of affairs. The current baseline (or status quo) is taken as a reference point, and any change from that baseline is perceived as a loss. Status quo bias should be distinguished from a rational preference for the status quo ante, as when the current state of affairs is objectively superior to the available alternatives, or when imperfect information is a significant problem. A large body of evidence, however, shows that status quo bias frequently affects human decision-making.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>

Conflicts of interestEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} A conflict of interest is when a person or association has intersecting interests (financial, personal, etc.) which could potentially corrupt. The potential conflict is autonomous of actual improper actions, it can be found and intentionally defused before corruption, or the appearance of corruption, happens. "A conflict of interest is a set of circumstances that creates a risk that professional judgement or actions regarding a primary interest will be unduly influenced by a secondary interest."<ref>Template:Cite book</ref> It exists if the circumstances are sensibly accepted to present a hazard that choices made may be unduly affected by auxiliary interests.<ref name=Cain>Template:Cite journal</ref>

CorruptionEdit

A conflict of interest arises when a decision-maker participates in a corrupt act that seeks to influence the outcome in favor of a specific individual, organization, or entity in a decision-making process.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> For example, attempts to solicit a bribe or kickback in exchange for favoring a party creates a conflict of interest.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> A perceived conflict of interest may also arise in an individual who is offered such a payment, even if it is declined, particularly in situations where the attempt to bribe is not reported.<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref>

Laws restricting monetary transaction is appropriate can differ between jurisdictions based upon their criminal laws. For example, some nations criminalize the receipt of political campaign contributions in the form of cash, while other nations permit cash donations provided that donors otherwise adhere to election law.Template:Citation needed

FavoritismEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Favoritism, sometimes known as in-group favoritism, or in-group bias, refers to a pattern of favoring members of one's in-group over out-group members. This can be expressed in evaluation of others, in allocation of resources, and in many other ways.<ref name=aronson>Aronson, E., Wilson, T. D., & Akert, R. (2010). Social psychology. 7th ed. Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall.</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> This has been researched by psychologists, especially social psychologists, and linked to group conflict and prejudice. Cronyism is favoritism of long-standing friends, especially by appointing them to positions of authority, regardless of their qualifications.<ref>Template:Cite dictionary</ref> Nepotism is favoritism granted to relatives.<ref>Template:Cite dictionary</ref><ref name="dictionary">"Nepotism." Template:Webarchive Dictionary.com. Retrieved 20 June 2013.</ref><ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref><ref name="Article Nepotism">{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref>

LobbyingEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}}

Lobbying is the attempt to influence choices made by administrators, frequently lawmakers or individuals from administrative agencies.<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref><ref>Template:Cite news</ref><ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref> Lobbyists may be among a legislator's constituencies, or not; they may engage in lobbying as a business, or not. Lobbying is often spoken of with contempt, the implication is that people with inordinate socioeconomic power are corrupting the law in order to serve their own interests. When people who have a duty to act on behalf of others, such as elected officials with a duty to serve their constituents' interests or more broadly the common good, stand to benefit by shaping the law to serve the interests of some private parties, there is a conflict of interest. This can lead to all sides in a debate looking to sway the issue by means of lobbyists.

Regulatory issuesEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Self-regulation is the process whereby an organization monitors its own adherence to legal, ethical, or safety standards, rather than have an outside, independent agency such as a third party entity monitor and enforce those standards.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> Self-regulation of any group can create a conflict of interest. If any organization, such as a corporation or government bureaucracy, is asked to eliminate unethical behavior within their own group, it may be in their interest in the short run to eliminate the appearance of unethical behavior, rather than the behavior itself.

Regulatory capture is a form of political corruption that can occur when a regulatory agency, created to act in the public interest, instead advances the commercial or political concerns of special interest groups that dominate the industry or sector it is charged with regulating.<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref><ref>Template:Cite news</ref> Regulatory capture occurs because groups or individuals with a high-stakes interest in the outcome of policy or regulatory decisions can be expected to focus their resources and energies in attempting to gain the policy outcomes they prefer, while members of the public, each with only a tiny individual stake in the outcome, will ignore it altogether.<ref name="nyt-tbl">Timothy B. Lee, "Entangling the Web" Template:Webarchive The New York Times (August 3, 2006). Retrieved April 1, 2011</ref> Regulatory capture is a risk to which a regulatory agency is exposed by its very nature.<ref>Gary Adams, Sharon Hayes, Stuart Weierter and John Boyd, "Regulatory Capture: Managing the Risk" Template:Webarchive ICE Australia, International Conferences and Events (PDF) (October 24, 2007). Retrieved April 14, 2011</ref><ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}, World Bank.</ref>

ShillingEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Shilling is deliberately giving spectators the feeling that one is an energetic autonomous client of a vendor for whom one is working. The effectiveness of shilling relies on crowd psychology to encourage other onlookers or audience members to purchase the goods or services (or accept the ideas being marketed). Shilling is illegal in some places, but legal in others.<ref>Template:Cite court</ref> An example of shilling is paid reviews that give the impression of being autonomous opinions.

Statistical biasesEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Statistical bias is a systematic tendency in the process of data collection, which results in lopsided, misleading results. This can occur in any of a number of ways, in the way the sample is selected, or in the way data are collected.<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref> It is a property of a statistical technique or of its results whereby the expected value of the results differs from the true underlying quantitative parameter being estimated.

Forecast biasEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} A forecast bias is when there are consistent differences between results and the forecasts of those quantities; that is: forecasts may have an overall tendency to be too high or too low.

Observer-expectancy effectEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} The observer-expectancy effect is when a researcher's expectations cause them to subconsciously influence the people participating in an experiment. It is usually controlled using a double-blind system, and was an important reason for the development of double-blind experiments.

Reporting bias and social desirability biasEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} In epidemiology and empirical research, reporting bias is defined as "selective revealing or suppression of information" of undesirable behavior by subjects<ref name="Porta2008">Template:Cite book</ref> or researchers.<ref>Green S, Higgins S, editors: Glossary. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions 4.2.5. Template:Webarchive</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> It refers to a tendency to under-report unexpected or undesirable experimental results, while being more trusting of expected or desirable results. This can propagate, as each instance reinforces the status quo, and later experimenters justify their own reporting bias by observing that previous experimenters reported different results.

Social desirability bias is a bias within social science research where survey respondents can tend to answer questions in a manner that will be viewed positively by others.<ref name="SDRdef">{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref> It can take the form of over-reporting laudable behavior, or under-reporting undesirable behavior. This bias interferes with the interpretation of average tendencies as well as individual differences. The inclination represents a major issue with self-report questionnaires; of special concern are self-reports of abilities, personalities, sexual behavior, and drug use.<ref name="SDRdef" />

Selection biasEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}}

File:Simple random sampling.PNG
Sampling is supposed to collect of a representative sample of a population.

Selection bias is the conscious or unconscious bias introduced into a study by the way individuals, groups or data are selected for analysis, if such a way means that true randomization is not achieved, thereby ensuring that the sample obtained is not representative of the population intended to be analyzed.<ref>Dictionary of Cancer Terms → selection bias Template:Webarchive. Retrieved on September 23, 2009.</ref> This results in a sample that may be significantly different from the overall population.

PrejudicesEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Bias and prejudice are usually considered to be closely related.<ref name=Merriam-Webster>"bias ...; prejudice", The New Merriam–Webster Dictionary, Template:ISBN</ref> Prejudice is prejudgment, or forming an opinion before becoming aware of the relevant facts of a case. The word is often used to refer to preconceived, usually unfavorable, judgments toward people or a person because of gender, political opinion, social class, age, disability, religion, sexuality, race/ethnicity, language, nationality, or other personal characteristics. Prejudice can also refer to unfounded beliefs<ref>William James wrote: "A great many people think they are thinking when they are merely rearranging their prejudices." Quotable Quotes – Courtesy of The Freeman Institute Template:Webarchive</ref> and may include "any unreasonable attitude that is unusually resistant to rational influence".<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>

AgeismEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Ageism is the stereotyping and/or discrimination against individuals or groups on the basis of their age. It can be used in reference to prejudicial attitudes towards older people, or towards younger people.

ClassismEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Classism is discrimination on the basis of social class. It includes attitudes that benefit the upper class at the expense of the lower class, or vice versa.<ref name=Kadi>Template:Cite book</ref>

LookismEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Lookism is stereotypes, prejudice, and discrimination on the basis of physical attractiveness, or more generally to people whose appearance matches cultural preferences.<ref>Template:Cite dictionary</ref><ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref><ref>Farrell, Warren (2005). Why Men Earn More: The Startling Truth About the Pay Gap -- And What Women Can Do About It. AMACOM, Template:ISBN p. 193</ref> Many people make automatic judgments of others based on their physical appearance that influence how they respond to those people.<ref name="Eagly et al., 1991).">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="Rhodes, Simmons, & Peters, 2005).">Template:Cite journal</ref>

RacismEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Racism consists of ideologies based on a desire to dominate or a belief in the inferiority of another race.<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> It may also hold that members of different races should be treated differently.<ref>Racism Template:Webarchive Oxford Dictionaries</ref><ref name="schaefer">"Racism" in R. Schefer. 2008 Encyclopedia of Race, Ethnicity and Society. SAGE. p. 1113</ref><ref name="newman">Template:Cite book</ref>

SexismEdit

Template:Excerpt

Contextual biasesEdit

Biases in academiaEdit

Template:See also

Academic biasEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Academic bias is the bias or perceived bias of scholars allowing their beliefs to shape their research and the scientific community. Claims of bias are often linked to claims by conservatives of pervasive bias against political conservatives and religious Christians.<ref>Template:Citation</ref> Some have argued that these claims are based upon anecdotal evidence which would not reliably indicate systematic bias,<ref name="Ames 2005">Template:Citation</ref><ref name="Lee 2006">Template:Citation</ref><ref>Template:Citation</ref> and have suggested that this divide is due to self-selection of conservatives choosing not to pursue academic careers.<ref name="Ames 2005"/><ref name=Gross2>Template:Citation</ref> There is some evidence that perception of classroom bias may be rooted in issues of sexuality, race, class and sex as much or more than in religion.<ref>Template:Citation</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>

Experimenter biasEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} In science research, experimenter bias occurs when experimenter expectancies regarding study results bias the research outcome.<ref name= Sackett>Template:Cite journal</ref> Examples of experimenter bias include conscious or unconscious influences on subject behavior including creation of demand characteristics that influence subjects, and altered or selective recording of experimental results themselves.<ref name="KantowitzIII2009">Template:Cite book</ref> It can also involve asking leading probes and not neutrally redirecting the subject back to the task when they ask for validation or questions.<ref name=":2">Template:Cite book</ref>

Funding biasEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Funding bias refers to the tendency of a scientific study to support the interests of the study's financial sponsor. This phenomenon is recognized sufficiently that researchers undertake studies to examine bias in past published studies.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> It can be caused by any or all of: a conscious or subconscious sense of obligation of researchers towards their employers,<ref>Template:Cite book</ref> misconduct or malpractice,<ref name="washingtonpost">Template:Cite news</ref> publication bias,<ref name="washingtonpost" /><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="lexchin">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref> or reporting bias.<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref>

Full text on net biasEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Full text on net (or FUTON) bias is a tendency of scholars to cite academic journals with open access—that is, journals that make their full text available on the internet without charge—in their own writing as compared with toll access publications. Scholars can more easily discover and access articles that have their full text on the internet, which increases authors' likelihood of reading, quoting, and citing these articles, this may increase the impact factor of open access journals relative to journals without open access.<ref name="pmid 15301326">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="pmid 12937253">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="cite pmid|16517987">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="cite pmid|18974812">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="pmid 12401287" />

The related bias, no abstract available bias (NAA bias) is scholars' tendency to cite journal articles that have an abstract available online more readily than articles that do not.<ref name="pmid 15301326" /><ref name="pmid 12401287">Template:Cite journal</ref>

Publication biasEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Publication bias is a type of bias with regard to what academic research is likely to be published because of a tendency among researchers and journal editors to prefer some outcomes rather than others (e.g., results showing a significant finding), which leads to a problematic bias in the published literature.<ref name="Song2010">Template:Cite journal</ref> This can propagate further as literature reviews of claims about support for a hypothesis will themselves be biased if the original literature is contaminated by publication bias.<ref name="Rothstein2005">H. Rothstein, A. J. Sutton and M. Borenstein. (2005). Publication bias in meta-analysis: prevention, assessment and adjustments. Wiley. Chichester, England; Hoboken, NJ.</ref> Studies with significant results often do not appear to be superior to studies with a null result with respect to quality of design.<ref name=Easterbrook>Template:Cite journal</ref> However, statistically significant results have been shown to be three times more likely to be published compared to papers with null results.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>

Biases in law enforcementEdit

Driving while blackEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Driving while black refers to the racial profiling of African American drivers. The phrase implies that a motorist might be pulled over by a police officer, questioned, and searched, because of a racial bias.<ref name=DWB_matters>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref><ref>Template:Cite magazine</ref>

Racial profilingEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Racial profiling, or ethnic profiling, is the act of suspecting or targeting a person of a certain race on the basis of racially observed characteristics or behavior, rather than on individual suspicion.<ref>Template:Cite encyclopedia</ref><ref name="Warren 2009 52–63">Template:Cite journal</ref> Racial profiling is commonly referred to regarding its use by law enforcement, and its leading to discrimination against minorities.

Victim blamingEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Victim blaming occurs when the victim of a wrongful act is held at fault for the harm that befell them.<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref> The study of victimology seeks to mitigate the perception of victims as responsible.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>

Biases in mediaEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Template:Anchor Media bias is the bias or perceived bias of journalists and news producers within the mass media in the selection of events, the stories that are reported, and how they are covered. The term generally implies a pervasive or widespread bias violating the standards of journalism, rather than the perspective of an individual journalist or article.<ref>Strategic Maneuvering and Media Bias in Political News Magazine Opinion Articles, Stefano Mario Rivolta, 7 June 2011</ref> The level of media bias in different nations is debated. There are also watchdog groups that report on media bias.

Practical limitations to media neutrality include the inability of journalists to report all available stories and facts, the requirement that selected facts be linked into a coherent narrative, government influence including overt and covert censorship,<ref>"10 Most Censored Countries" Template:Webarchive, Committee to Protect Journalists, 2 May 2006</ref> the influence of the owners of the news source, concentration of media ownership, the selection of staff, the preferences of an intended audience, and pressure from advertisers.

Bias has been a feature of the mass media since its birth with the invention of the printing press. The expense of early printing equipment restricted media production to a limited number of people. Historians have found that publishers often served the interests of powerful social groups.<ref>Ann Heinrichs, The Printing Press (Inventions That Shaped the World), p. 53, Franklin Watts, 2005, Template:ISBN, Template:ISBN</ref>

Agenda settingEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Agenda setting describes the capacity of the media to focus on particular stories, if a news item is covered frequently and prominently, the audience will regard the issue as more important. That is, its salience will increase.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>

GatekeepingEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Gatekeeping is the way in which information and news are filtered to the public, by each person or corporation along the way. It is the "process of culling and crafting countless bits of information into the limited number of messages that reach people every day, and it is the center of the media's role in modern public life. [...] This process determines not only which information is selected, but also what the content and nature of the messages, such as news, will be."<ref>Template:Cite book</ref>

SensationalismEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Sensationalism is when events and topics in news stories and pieces are overhyped to present skewed impressions of events, which may cause a misrepresentation of the truth of a story.<ref name="fair">"Issue Area: Sensationalism." Fairness and Accuracy In Reporting. Accessed June 2011.</ref> Sensationalism may involve reporting about insignificant matters and events, or the presentation of newsworthy topics in a trivial or tabloid manner contrary to the standards of professional journalism.<ref name="stephens">Template:Cite book</ref><ref name="thompson">Template:Cite book</ref>

Other contextsEdit

Educational biasEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Bias in education refers to real or perceived bias in the educational system. The content of school textbooks is often the issue of debate, as their target audience is young people, and the term "whitewashing" is used to refer to selective removal of critical or damaging evidence or comment.<ref name="7forms">{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref><ref name="Washpost">Template:Cite news</ref><ref name="whitewashhist">{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref> Religious bias in textbooks is observed in countries where religion plays a dominant role. There can be many forms of educational bias. Some overlooked aspects, occurring especially with the pedagogical circles of public and private schools—sources that are unrelated to fiduciary or mercantile impoverishment which may be unduly magnified—include teacher bias as well as a general bias against women who are going into STEM research.<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref><ref>"Crisis Counseling with Children," Van Ornum and Murdock, 1990, NY: Crossroad/Continuum.</ref>

Inductive biasEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Inductive bias occurs within the field of machine learning. In machine learning one seeks to develop algorithms that are able to learn to anticipate a particular output. To accomplish this, the learning algorithm is given training cases that show the expected connection. Then the learner is tested with new examples. Without further assumptions, this problem cannot be solved exactly as unknown situations may not be predictable.<ref name=Mitchell1980 /><ref name=DesJardinsandGordon1995>Template:Cite journal</ref> The inductive bias of the learning algorithm is the set of assumptions that the learner uses to predict outputs given inputs that it has not encountered.<ref name=Mitchell1980>Template:Cite report</ref> It may bias the learner towards the correct solution, the incorrect, or be correct some of the time. A classical example of an inductive bias is Occam's Razor, which assumes that the simplest consistent hypothesis is the best.

Insider tradingEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Insider trading is the trading of a public company's stock or other securities (such as bonds or stock options) by individuals with access to non-public information about the company. In various countries, trading based on insider information is illegal because it is seen as unfair to other investors who do not have access to the information as the investor with insider information could potentially make far larger profits that a typical investor could make.

Match fixingEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} In organized sports, match fixing occurs when a match is played to a completely or partially pre-determined result, violating the rules of the game and often the law.<ref>Template:Cite dictionary</ref> There is a variety of reasons for this, but the most common is in exchange for a payoff from gamblers. Players might also intentionally perform poorly to get an advantage in the future (such as a better draft pick, or an easier opponent in a playoff), or to rig a handicap system. Match-fixing generally refers to fixing the final result of the game. Another form of match-fixing, known as spot-fixing, involves fixing small events within a match which can be gambled upon, but which are unlikely to prove decisive in determining the final result of the game.

Implicit biasEdit

{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}}An implicit bias, or implicit stereotype, is the unconscious attribution of particular qualities to a member of a certain social group.<ref name="GandB95">Template:Cite journal</ref>

Implicit stereotypes are shaped by experience and based on learned associations between particular qualities and social categories, including race and/or gender. Individuals' perceptions and behaviors can be influenced by the implicit stereotypes they hold, even if they are unaware/unintentionally hold such stereotypes. Implicit bias is an aspect of implicit social cognition: the phenomenon that perceptions, attitudes, and stereotypes operate without conscious intention. For example, researchers may have implicit bias when designing survey questions and as a result, the questions do not produce accurate results or fail to encourage survey participation.<ref name=":2" /> The existence of implicit bias is supported by a variety of scientific articles in psychological literature. Implicit stereotype was first defined by psychologists Mahzarin Banaji and Anthony Greenwald in 1995.

See alsoEdit

Template:Portal Template:Columns-list

ReferencesEdit

Template:Reflist

External linksEdit

Template:Sister project links Template:Biases Template:Conflict of interest Template:Discrimination Template:Philosophical logic