Motion sickness
Template:Short description {{#invoke:other uses|otheruses}} Template:Infobox medical condition (new) Motion sickness occurs due to a difference between actual and expected motion.<ref name=HaleStanney2014/><ref name=Stat2019/><ref name=ReasonBrand1975/> Symptoms commonly include nausea, vomiting, cold sweat, headache, dizziness, tiredness, loss of appetite, and increased salivation.<ref name=HaleStanney2014/><ref name=Hromatka2015>Template:Cite journal</ref> Complications may rarely include dehydration, electrolyte problems, or a lower esophageal tear.<ref name=Stat2019/>
The cause of motion sickness is either real or perceived motion.<ref name=Stat2019/> This may include car travel, air travel, sea travel, space travel, or reality simulation.<ref name=Stat2019/> Risk factors include pregnancy, migraines, and Ménière's disease.<ref name=Stat2019/> The diagnosis is based on symptoms.<ref name=Stat2019/>
Treatment may include behavioral measures or medications.<ref name=Gold2016/> Behavioral measures include keeping the head still and focusing on the horizon.<ref name=Hemmerich2020/><ref name=Munafo2016/> Three types of medications are useful: antimuscarinics such as scopolamine, H1 antihistamines such as dimenhydrinate, and amphetamines such as dexamphetamine.<ref name=Gold2016>Template:Cite book</ref> Side effects, however, may limit the use of medications.<ref name=Gold2016/> A number of medications used for nausea such as ondansetron are not effective for motion sickness.<ref name=Gold2016/>
Many people can be affected with sufficient motion<ref name=Stat2019/> and some people will experience motion sickness at least once in their lifetime.<ref name=Herron2010/> Susceptibility, however, is variable, with about one-third of the population being susceptible while other people can be affected only under very extreme conditions.<ref name=Stat2019/> Women can be more easily affected than men.<ref name=Hemmerich2019/> Motion sickness has been described since at least the time of Homer (Template:Circa eighth century BC).<ref name=Huppert2017/> Template:TOC limit
Signs and symptomsEdit
Symptoms commonly include nausea, vomiting, cold sweat, headache, dizziness, tiredness, loss of appetite, and increased salivation.<ref name=Stat2019/><ref name=Hromatka2015/> Occasionally, tiredness can last for hours to days after an episode of motion sickness, known as "sopite syndrome".<ref name=Stat2019/> Rarely severe symptoms such as the inability to walk, ongoing vomiting, or social isolation may occur while rare complications may include dehydration, electrolyte problems, or a lower esophageal tear from severe vomiting.<ref name=Stat2019/>
CauseEdit
Motion sickness can be divided into three categories:Template:Citation needed
- Motion sickness caused by motion that is felt but not seen i.e. terrestrial motion sickness;
- Motion sickness caused by motion that is seen but not felt i.e. space motion sickness;
- Motion sickness caused when both systems detect motion but they do not correspond i.e. either terrestrial or space motion sickness.
Motion felt but not seenEdit
In these cases, motion is sensed by the vestibular system and hence the motion is felt, but no motion or little motion is detected by the visual system, as in terrestrial motion sickness.Template:Citation needed
CarsicknessEdit
Template:More citations needed A specific form of terrestrial motion sickness, being carsick is quite common and evidenced by disorientation while reading a map, a book, or a small screen during travel. Carsickness results from the sensory conflict arising in the brain from differing sensory inputs. Motion sickness is caused by a conflict between signals arriving in the brain from the inner ear, which forms the base of the vestibular system, the sensory apparatus that deals with movement and balance, and which detects motion mechanically. If someone is looking at a stationary object within a vehicle, such as a magazine, their eyes will inform their brain that what they are viewing is not moving. Their inner ears, however, will contradict this by sensing the motion of the vehicle.<ref name="economist2018"/>
Varying theories exist as to cause. The sensory conflict theory notes that the eyes view motion while riding in the moving vehicle while other body sensors sense stillness, creating conflict between the eyes and inner ear. Another suggests the eyes mostly see the interior of the car which is motionless while the vestibular system of the inner ear senses motion as the vehicle goes around corners or over hills and even small bumps. Therefore, the effect is worse when looking down but may be lessened by looking outside of the vehicle.
In the early 20th century, Austro-Hungarian scientist Róbert Bárány observed the back and forth movement of the eyes of railroad passengers as they looked out the side windows at the scenery whipping by. He called this "railway nystagmus", also known as "optokinetic nystagmus". His findings were published in the journal Laeger, 83:1516, Nov.17, 1921.
AirsicknessEdit
{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Air sickness is a kind of terrestrial motion sickness induced by certain sensations of air travel.<ref name="Benson 2002" /> It is a specific form of motion sickness and is considered a normal response in healthy individuals. It is essentially the same as carsickness but occurs in an airplane. An airplane may bank and tilt sharply, and unless passengers are sitting by a window, they are likely to see only the stationary interior of the plane due to the small window sizes and during flights at night. Another factor is that while in flight, the view out of windows may be blocked by clouds, preventing passengers from seeing the moving ground or passing clouds.Template:Citation needed
SeasicknessEdit
Seasickness is a form of terrestrial motion sickness characterized by a feeling of nausea and, in extreme cases, vertigo experienced after spending time on a boat.<ref name="Benson 2002"/> It is essentially the same as carsickness, though the motion of a watercraft tends to be more regular. It is typically brought on by the rocking motion of the craft<ref name="Gahlinger 2000" /><ref name="Sharma1992" /> or movement while the craft is immersed in water.<ref name="Norfleet et al 1992" /> As with airsickness, it can be difficult to visually detect motion even if one looks outside the boat since water does not offer fixed points with which to visually judge motion. Poor visibility conditions, such as fog, may worsen seasickness. The greatest contributor to seasickness is the tendency for people being affected by the rolling or surging motions of the craft to seek refuge below decks, where they are unable to relate themselves to the boat's surroundings and consequent motion. Some people with carsickness are resistant to seasickness and vice versa.Template:Citation needed Adjusting to the craft's motion at sea is called "gaining one's sea legs"; it can take a significant portion of the time spent at sea after disembarking to regain a sense of stability "post-sea legs".
Centrifuge motion sicknessEdit
Rotating devices such as centrifuges used in astronaut training and amusement park rides such as the Rotor, Mission: Space and the Gravitron can cause motion sickness in many people. While the interior of the centrifuge does not appear to move, one will experience a sense of motion.{{ safesubst:#invoke:Unsubst||date=__DATE__ |$B= Template:Fix }} In addition, centrifugal force can cause the vestibular system to give one the sense that downward is in the direction away from the center of the centrifuge rather than the true downward direction.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
Dizziness due to spinningEdit
When one spins and stops suddenly, fluid in the inner ear continues to rotate causing a sense of continued spinning while one's visual system no longer detects motion.Template:Citation needed
Virtual realityEdit
{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Usually, VR programs would detect the motion of the user's head and adjust the rotation of vision to avoid dizziness. However, some cases such as system lagging or software crashing could cause lags in the screen updates. In such cases, even some small head motions could trigger the motion sickness by the defense mechanism mentioned below: the inner ear transmits to the brain that it senses motion, but the eyes tell the brain that everything is still.
Motion seen but not feltEdit
In these cases, motion is detected by the visual system and hence the motion is seen, but no motion or little motion is sensed by the vestibular system. Motion sickness arising from such situations has been referred to as "visually induced motion sickness" (VIMS).<ref name="So and Ujike 2010" />
Space motion sicknessEdit
{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}} Zero gravity interferes with the vestibular system's gravity-dependent operations, so that the two systems, vestibular and visual, no longer provide a unified and coherent sensory representation. This causes unpleasant disorientation sensations often quite distinct from terrestrial motion sickness, but with similar symptoms. The symptoms may be more intense because a condition caused by prolonged weightlessness is usually quite unfamiliar.Template:Citation needed
Space motion sickness was effectively unknown during the earliest spaceflights because the very cramped conditions of the spacecraft allowed for only minimal bodily motion, especially head motion. Space motion sickness seems to be aggravated by being able to freely move around, and so is more common in larger spacecraft.<ref name="Benson 2002" /> Around 60% of Space Shuttle astronauts experienced it on their first flight; the first case of space motion sickness is now thought to be the Soviet cosmonaut Gherman Titov, in August 1961 onboard Vostok 2, who reported dizziness, nausea, and vomiting. The first severe cases were in early Apollo flights; Frank Borman on Apollo 8 and Rusty Schweickart on Apollo 9. Both experienced identifiable and quite unpleasant symptoms—in the latter case causing the mission plan to be modified.Template:Citation needed
Screen imagesEdit
This type of terrestrial motion sickness is particularly prevalent when susceptible people are watching films presented on very large screens such as IMAX, but may also occur in regular format theaters or even when watching TV or playing games. For the sake of novelty, IMAX and other panoramic type theaters often show dramatic motions such as flying over a landscape or riding a roller coaster.
In regular-format theaters, an example of a movie that caused motion sickness in many people is The Blair Witch Project. Theaters warned patrons of its possible nauseating effects, cautioning pregnant women in particular. Blair Witch was filmed with a handheld camcorder, which was subjected to considerably more motion than the average movie camera,<ref name="Wax" /> and lacks the stabilization mechanisms of steadicams.
Home movies, often filmed with a cell phone camera, also tend to cause motion sickness in those who view them. The person holding the cell phone or other camera usually is unaware of this as the recording is being made since the sense of motion seems to match the motion seen through the camera's viewfinder. Those who view the film afterward only see the movement, which may be considerable, without any sense of motion. Using the zoom function seems to contribute to motion sickness as well since zooming is not a normal function of the eye. The use of a tripod or a camera or cell phone with image stabilization while filming can reduce this effect.Template:Nosource
Virtual realityEdit
Template:See also Motion sickness due to virtual reality is very similar to simulation sickness and motion sickness due to films.<ref name="ARVI"/> In virtual reality the effect is made more acute as all external reference points are blocked from vision, the simulated images are three-dimensional and in some cases stereo sound that may also give a sense of motion. The NADS-1, a simulator located at the National Advanced Driving Simulator, is capable of accurately stimulating the vestibular system with a 360-degree horizontal field of view and 13 degrees of freedom motion base.<ref name="NADS"/> Studies have shown that exposure to rotational motions in a virtual environment can cause significant increases in nausea and other symptoms of motion sickness.<ref name="SoLo1999"/>
In a study conducted by the U.S. Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences in a report published May 1995 titled "Technical Report 1027 – Simulator Sickness in Virtual Environments", out of 742 pilot exposures from 11 military flight simulators, "approximately half of the pilots (334) reported post-effects of some kind: 250 (34%) reported that symptoms dissipated in less than one hour, 44 (6%) reported that symptoms lasted longer than four hours, and 28 (4%) reported that symptoms lasted longer than six hours. There were also four (1%) reported cases of spontaneously occurring flashbacks."<ref name="cyberedge" />
Motion that is seen and feltEdit
When moving within a rotating reference frame such as in a centrifuge or environment where gravity is simulated with centrifugal force, the coriolis effect causes a sense of motion in the vestibular system that does not match the motion that is seen.Template:Citation needed
PathophysiologyEdit
There are various hypotheses that attempt to explain the cause of the condition.
Sensory conflict theoryEdit
Contemporary sensory conflict theory, referring to "a discontinuity between either visual, proprioceptive, and somatosensory input, or semicircular canal and otolith input", is probably the most thoroughly studied.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> According to this theory, when the brain presents the mind with two incongruous states of motion, the result is often nausea and other symptoms of disorientation known as motion sickness.Template:Why Such conditions happen when the vestibular system and the visual system do not present a synchronized and unified representation of one's body and surroundings.<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref>
According to sensory conflict theory, the cause of terrestrial motion sickness is the opposite of the cause of space motion sickness. The former occurs when one perceives visually that one's surroundings are relatively immobile while the vestibular system reports that one's body is in motion relative to its surroundings.<ref name="Benson 2002" /> The latter can occur when the visual system perceives that one's surroundings are in motion while the vestibular system reports relative bodily immobility (as in zero gravity.)Template:Citation needed
Neural mismatchEdit
A variation of the sensory conflict theory is known as neural mismatch, implying a mismatch occurring between ongoing sensory experience and long-term memory rather than between components of the vestibular and visual systems. This theory emphasizes "the limbic system in the integration of sensory information and long-term memory, in the expression of the symptoms of motion sickness, and the impact of anti-motion-sickness drugs and stress hormones on limbic system function. The limbic system may be the neural mismatch center of the brain."<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
Defense against poisoningEdit
It has also been proposed that motion sickness could function as a defense mechanism against neurotoxins.<ref name="Treisman1977" /> The area postrema in the brain is responsible for inducing vomiting when poisons are detected, and for resolving conflicts between vision and balance. When feeling motion but not seeing it (for example, in the cabin of a ship with no portholes), the inner ear transmits to the brain that it senses motion, but the eyes tell the brain that everything is still. As a result of the incongruity, the brain concludes that the individual is hallucinating and further concludes that the hallucination is due to poison ingestion. The brain responds by inducing vomiting, to clear the supposed toxin. Treisman's indirect argument has recently been questioned via an alternative direct evolutionary hypothesis, as well as modified and extended via a direct poison hypothesis.<ref name="Lawson 20142"/> The direct evolutionary hypothesis essentially argues that there are plausible means by which ancient real or apparent motion could have contributed directly to the evolution of aversive reactions, without the need for the co-opting of a poison response as posited by Treisman. Nevertheless, the direct poison hypothesis argues that there still are plausible ways in which the body's poison response system may have played a role in shaping the evolution of some of the signature symptoms that characterize motion sickness.Template:Citation needed
Nystagmus hypothesisEdit
Yet another theory, known as the nystagmus hypothesis,<ref name="Ebenholtz et al 1994" /> has been proposed based on stimulation of the vagus nerve resulting from the stretching or traction of extra-ocular muscles co-occurring with eye movements caused by vestibular stimulation. There are three critical aspects to the theory: first is the close linkage between activity in the vestibular system, i.e., semicircular canals and otolith organs, and a change in tonus among various of each eye's six extra-ocular muscles. Thus, with the exception of voluntary eye movements, the vestibular and oculomotor systems are thoroughly linked. Second is the operation of Sherrington's Law<ref name="Sherrington1893" /> describing reciprocal inhibition between agonist-antagonist muscle pairs, and by implication the stretching of extraocular muscle that must occur whenever Sherrington's Law is made to fail, thereby causing an unrelaxed (contracted) muscle to be stretched. Finally, there is the critical presence of afferent output to the Vagus nerves as a direct result of eye muscle stretch or traction.<ref name="pmid6671149" /> Thus, tenth nerve stimulation resulting from eye muscle stretch is proposed as the cause of motion sickness. The theory explains why labyrinthine-defective individuals are immune to motion sickness;<ref name="Kennedy et al 1968" /><ref name="pmid1859339" /> why symptoms emerge when undergoing various body-head accelerations; why combinations of voluntary and reflexive eye movements may challenge the proper operation of Sherrington's Law, and why many drugs that suppress eye movements also serve to suppress motion sickness symptoms.<ref name="Ebenholtz 2005" />
A recent theory <ref name="Previc 2018" /> argues that the main reason motion sickness occurs is due to an imbalance in vestibular outputs favoring the semicircular canals (nauseogenic) vs. otolith organs (anti-nauseogenic). This theory attempts to integrate previous theories of motion sickness. For example, there are many sensory conflicts that are associated with motion sickness and many that are not, but those in which canal stimulation occurs in the absence of normal otolith function (e.g., in free fall) are the most provocative. The vestibular imbalance theory is also tied to the different roles of the otoliths and canals in autonomic arousal (otolith output more sympathetic).Template:Citation needed
DiagnosisEdit
The diagnosis is based on symptoms.<ref name=Stat2019/> Other conditions that may present similarly include vestibular disorders such as benign paroxysmal positional vertigo and vestibular migraine and stroke.<ref name=Stat2019/>
TreatmentEdit
Treatment may include behavioral measures or medications.<ref name=Gold2016/>
Behavioral measuresEdit
Behavioral measures to decrease motion sickness include holding the head still and lying on the back.<ref name=Gold2016/> Focusing on the horizon may also be useful.<ref name=Stat2019/> Listening to music, mindful breathing, being the driver, and not reading while moving are other techniques.<ref name=Stat2019/>
Habituation is the most effective technique but requires significant time.<ref name=Stat2019/> It is often used by the military for pilots.<ref name=Stat2019/> These techniques must be carried out at least every week to retain effectiveness.<ref name=Stat2019/>
A head-worn, computer device with a transparent display can be used to mitigate the effects of motion sickness (and spatial disorientation) if visual indicators of the wearer's head position are shown.<ref name="pmid21181963"/> Such a device functions by providing the wearer with digital reference lines in their field of vision that indicate the horizon's position relative to the user's head. This is accomplished by combining readings from accelerometers and gyroscopes mounted in the device. This technology has been implemented in both standalone devices<ref name="biz"/> and Google Glass.<ref name="BCM"/><ref name="glass"/> One promising looking treatment is to wear LCD shutter glasses that create a stroboscopic vision of 4 Hz with a dwell of 10 milliseconds.<ref name="ntrs" >Template:Cite journal</ref>
MedicationEdit
Three types of medications are sometimes prescribed to improve symptoms of motion sickness: antimuscarinics such as scopolamine, H1 antihistamines such as dimenhydrinate, and amphetamines such as dexamphetamine.<ref name=Gold2016/> Benefits are greater if used before the onset of symptoms or shortly after symptoms begin.<ref name=Stat2019/> Side effects, however, may limit the use of medications.<ref name=Gold2016/> A number of medications used for nausea such as ondansetron and metoclopramide are not effective in motion sickness.<ref name=Gold2016/><ref name=Stat2019/>
Scopolamine (antimuscarinic)Edit
Scopolamine is the most effective medication.<ref name="Stat2019" /> Evidence is best for when it is used preventatively.<ref name="Spinks2011">Template:Cite journal</ref> It is available as a skin patch.<ref name="Stat2019" /> Side effects may include blurry vision.<ref name="Stat2019" />
AntihistaminesEdit
First generation H1 antihistamine medications are sometimes given to prevent or treat motion sickness. This class of medication is often effective at reducing the risk of getting motion sickness while in motion, however, the effectiveness of antihistamines at treating or stopping motion sickness once a person is already experiencing it has not been well studied.<ref name=":0">Template:Cite journal</ref> Effective first generation antihistamines include doxylamine, diphenhydramine, promethazine, meclizine, cyclizine, and cinnarizine.<ref name="Stat2019" /> In pregnancy meclizine, dimenhydrinate and doxylamine are generally felt to be safe.<ref name="Stat2019" /> Side effects include sleepiness.<ref name="Stat2019" /><ref name=":0" /> Second generation antihistamines have not been found to be useful.<ref name="Stat2019" />
Some, but not all, antihistamines used for motion sickness are also antimuscarinics.<ref name="Stat2019" />
AmphetaminesEdit
Dextroamphetamine may be used together with an antihistamine or an antimuscarinic with synergistic effects.<ref name=Stat2019/> Concerns include their addictive potential.<ref name=Stat2019/>
Those involved in high-risk activities, such as SCUBA diving, should evaluate the risks versus the benefits of medications.<ref name="Schwartz and Curley1986" /><ref name="Lawson et al 2009" /><ref name="Bitterman et al 1991" /><ref name="Williams et al 1988" /><ref name="Arieli et al 1999" /> Promethazine combined with ephedrine to counteract the sedation is known as "the Coast Guard cocktail".<ref name="ECU" />
Alternative medicineEdit
Alternative treatments include acupuncture and ginger, although their effectiveness against motion sickness is variable.<ref name=Gold2016/><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="Brainard et al 2014" /><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> Providing smells does not appear to have a significant effect on the rate of motion sickness.<ref name=Gold2016/>
EpidemiologyEdit
Roughly one-third of people are highly susceptible to motion sickness, and most of the rest get motion sick under extreme conditions. Around 80% of the general population is susceptible to cases of medium to high motion sickness. The rates of space motion sickness have been estimated at between forty and eighty percent of those who enter weightless orbit. Several factors influence susceptibility to motion sickness, including sleep deprivation and the cubic footage allocated to each space traveler. Studies indicate that women are more likely to be affected than men,<ref name=Stat2019/> and that the risk decreases with advancing age. There is some evidence that people with Asian ancestry may develop motion sickness more frequently than people of European ancestry, and there are situational and behavioral factors, such as whether a passenger has a view of the road ahead, and diet and eating behaviors.<ref name="pmid25628336"/>
See alsoEdit
- Mal de debarquement - disembarkment syndrome, usually follows a cruise or other motion experience
ReferencesEdit
External linksEdit
Template:Sister project Template:Sister project
Template:Motion sickness Template:Consequences of external causes Template:Underwater diving