Placebo
Template:Short description Template:Cs1 config {{#invoke:other uses|otheruses}}
A placebo (Template:IPAc-en Template:Respell) can be roughly defined as a sham medical treatment.<ref name=aspmn/> Common placebos include inert tablets (like sugar pills), inert injections (like saline), sham surgery,<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref> and other procedures.<ref name="Lanotte">Template:Cite journal</ref>
Placebos are used in randomized clinical trials to test the efficacy of medical treatments. In a placebo-controlled clinical trial, any change in the control group is known as the placebo response, and the difference between this and the result of no treatment is the placebo effect.<ref name="chaplin" /> Placebos in clinical trials should ideally be indistinguishable from so-called verum treatments under investigation, except for the latter's particular hypothesized medicinal effect.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> This is to shield test participants (with their consent) from knowing who is getting the placebo and who is getting the treatment under test, as patients' and clinicians' expectations of efficacy can influence results.<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref><ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref>
The idea of a placebo effect was discussed in 18th century psychology,<ref>Schwarz, K. A., & Pfister, R.: Scientific psychology in the 18th century: a historical rediscovery. In: Perspectives on Psychological Science, Nr. 11, pp. 399–407.</ref> but became more prominent in the 20th century. Modern studies find that placebos can affect some outcomes such as pain and nausea, but otherwise do not generally have important clinical effects.<ref name="CochraneHrob2010" /> Improvements that patients experience after being treated with a placebo can also be due to unrelated factors, such as regression to the mean (a statistical effect where an unusually high or low measurement is likely to be followed by a less extreme one).<ref name="acs" /> The use of placebos in clinical medicine raises ethical concerns, especially if they are disguised as an active treatment, as this introduces dishonesty into the doctor–patient relationship and bypasses informed consent.<ref name="Newman">Template:Cite book</ref>
Placebos are also popular because they can sometimes produce relief through psychological mechanisms (a phenomenon known as the "placebo effect"). They can affect how patients perceive their condition and encourage the body's chemical processes for relieving pain<ref name="acs">{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref> and a few other symptoms,<ref name="quattrone-barbagallo" /> but have no impact on the disease itself.<ref name="CochraneHrob2010" /><ref name="acs" />
EtymologyEdit
The Latin term {{#invoke:Lang|lang}} (pronounced {{#invoke:IPA|main}} or {{#invoke:IPA|main}}) means [I] shall be pleasing. It was used as a name for the Vespers in the Office of the Dead, taken from its incipit, a quote from the Vulgate's Psalm 116:9 (Psalm 114:9 in modern bibles), {{#invoke:Lang|lang}}, "[I] shall please the Lord in the land of the living".<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref><ref>Template:Bibleverse</ref><ref name=Jacobs00>Template:Cite journal</ref> From that, a singer of placebo became associated with someone who falsely claimed a connection to the deceased to get a share of the funeral meal, and hence a flatterer, and so a deceptive act to please.<ref name="Shapiro"/>
DefinitionsEdit
The definition of placebo has been debated.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> One definition states that a treatment process is a placebo when none of the characteristic treatment factors are effective (remedial or harmful) in the patient for a given disease.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
In a clinical trial, a placebo response is the measured response of subjects to a placebo; the placebo effect is the difference between that response and no treatment.<ref name=chaplin>Template:Cite journal</ref> The placebo response may include improvements due to natural healing, declines due to natural disease progression, the tendency for people who were temporarily feeling either better or worse than usual to return to their average situations (regression toward the mean), and errors in the clinical trial records, which can make it appear that a change has happened when nothing has changed.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> It is also part of the recorded response to any active medical intervention.<ref name=eccles>Template:Cite journal</ref>
Measurable placebo effects may be either objective (e.g. lowered blood pressure) or subjective (e.g. a lowered perception of pain).<ref name="aspmn">Template:Cite journal</ref>
EffectsEdit
Template:Redirect Template:Further
Placebo can improve patient-reported outcomes such as pain and nausea.<ref name=CochraneHrob2010>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> A 2001 meta-analysis of the placebo effect looked at trials in 40 different medical conditions, and concluded the only one where it had been shown to have a significant effect was for pain.<ref name="Hrob2001" /> Another Cochrane review in 2010 suggested that placebo effects are apparent only in subjective, continuous measures, and in the treatment of pain and related conditions. The review found that placebos do not appear to affect the actual diseases, or outcomes that are not dependent on a patient's perception. The authors, Asbjørn Hróbjartsson and Peter C. Gøtzsche, concluded that their study "did not find that placebo interventions have important clinical effects in general".<ref name="CochraneHrob2010" /> This interpretation has been subject to criticism, as the existence of placebo effects seems undeniable.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> For example, recent research has linked placebo interventions to improved motor functions in patients with Parkinson's disease.<ref name="quattrone-barbagallo">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> Other objective outcomes affected by placebos include immune and endocrine parameters,<ref name=":1">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="Pacheco" /> end-organ functions regulated by the autonomic nervous system,<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> and sport performance.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
Measuring the extent of the placebo effect is difficult due to confounding factors.<ref name=Hrob2004>Template:Cite journal Gøtzsche's biographical article has further references related to this work.</ref> For example, a patient may feel better after taking a placebo due to regression to the mean (i.e. a natural recovery or change in symptoms),<ref name="Kienle & Kiene">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="How much?">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name=RTM>Template:Cite journal</ref> but this can be ruled out by comparing the placebo group with a no treatment group (as all the placebo research does). It is harder still to tell the difference between the placebo effect and the effects of response bias, observer bias and other flaws in trial methodology, as a trial comparing placebo treatment and no treatment will not be a blinded experiment.<ref name=CochraneHrob2010/><ref name="Kienle & Kiene"/> In their 2010 meta-analysis of the placebo effect, Asbjørn Hróbjartsson and Peter C. Gøtzsche argue that "even if there were no true effect of placebo, one would expect to record differences between placebo and no-treatment groups due to bias associated with lack of blinding".<ref name=CochraneHrob2010/>
One way in which the magnitude of placebo analgesia can be measured is by conducting "open/hidden" studies, in which some patients receive an analgesic and are informed that they will be receiving it (open), while others are administered the same drug without their knowledge (hidden). Such studies have found that analgesics are considerably more effective when the patient knows they are receiving them.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
Factors influencing the power of the placebo effectEdit
A review published in JAMA Psychiatry found that, in trials of antipsychotic medications, the change in response to receiving a placebo had increased significantly between 1960 and 2013. The review's authors identified several factors that could be responsible for this change, including inflation of baseline scores and enrollment of fewer severely ill patients.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> Another analysis published in Pain in 2015 found that placebo responses had increased considerably in neuropathic pain clinical trials conducted in the United States from 1990 to 2013. The researchers suggested that this may be because such trials have "increased in study size and length" during this time period.<ref>Template:Cite journal
- Template:Lay source</ref>
Individual differences in personality traits may influence susceptibility to placebo and nocebo (negative placebo) effects. People with a more optimistic outlook tend to exhibit stronger placebo responses, while those with higher levels of anxiety are more likely to experience nocebo effects.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
Children seem to have a greater response than adults to placebos.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
The administration of the placebos can determine the placebo effect strength. Studies have found that taking more pills would strengthen the effect. Capsules appear to be more influential than pills, and injections are even stronger than capsules.<ref name="Rosenberg2010_176">Template:Cite book</ref>
Some studies have investigated the use of placebos where the patient is fully aware that the treatment is inert, known as an open-label placebo. Clinical trials found that open-label placebos may have positive effects in comparison to no treatment, which may open new avenues for treatments,<ref name="Blease2019">Template:Cite journal</ref> but a review of such trials noted that they were done with a small number of participants and hence should be interpreted with "caution" until further, better-controlled trials are conducted.<ref name="onlinelibrary.wiley.com">Template:Cite journal</ref> An updated 2021 systematic review and meta-analysis based on 11 studies also found a significant, albeit slightly smaller overall effect of open-label placebos, while noting that "research on OLPs is still in its infancy".<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
If the person dispensing the placebo shows their care towards the patient, is friendly and sympathetic, or has a high expectation of a treatment's success, then the placebo is more effectual.<ref name="Rosenberg2010_176" />
DepressionEdit
In 2008, a meta-analysis led by psychologist Irving Kirsch, analyzing data from the Food and Drug Administration (FDA), concluded that 82% of the response to antidepressants was accounted for by placebos.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> However, other authors expressed doubts about the used methods and the interpretation of the results, especially the use of 0.5 as the cut-off point for the effect size.<ref name="TurnerRosenthal2008">Template:Cite journal</ref> A complete reanalysis and recalculation based on the same FDA data found that the Kirsch study had "important flaws in the calculations".<ref name="FountoulakisMöller2010" /> The authors concluded that although a large percentage of the placebo response was due to expectancy, this was not true for the active drug.<ref name="FountoulakisMöller2010" /> Besides confirming drug effectiveness, they found that the drug effect was not related to depression severity.<ref name="FountoulakisMöller2010">Template:Cite journal</ref>
Another meta-analysis found that 79% of depressed patients receiving placebo remained well (for 12 weeks after an initial 6–8 weeks of successful therapy) compared to 93% of those receiving antidepressants. In the continuation phase however, patients on placebo relapsed significantly more often than patients on antidepressants.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
PsychedelicsEdit
It has been proposed that psychedelics used for therapeutic purposes may act as active "super placebos".<ref>Template:Cite news</ref><ref name="DupuisVeissière2022">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="vanElkYaden2022">Template:Cite journal</ref>
Negative effectsEdit
{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}}
A phenomenon opposite to the placebo effect has also been observed. When an inactive substance or treatment is administered to a recipient who has an expectation of it having a negative impact, this intervention is known as a nocebo (Latin {{#invoke:Lang|lang}} = "I shall harm").<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref> A nocebo effect occurs when the recipient of an inert substance reports a negative effect or a worsening of symptoms, with the outcome resulting not from the substance itself, but from negative expectations about the treatment.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="autogenerated4">{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref>
Another negative consequence is that placebos can cause side-effects associated with real treatment.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
Withdrawal symptoms can also occur after placebo treatment. This was found, for example, after the discontinuation of the Women's Health Initiative study of hormone replacement therapy for menopause. Women had been on placebo for an average of 5.7 years. Moderate or severe withdrawal symptoms were reported by 4.8% of those on placebo compared to 21.3% of those on hormone replacement.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
EthicsEdit
In research trialsEdit
Knowingly giving a person a placebo when there is an effective treatment available is a bioethically complex issue. While placebo-controlled trials might provide information about the effectiveness of a treatment, it denies some patients what could be the best available (if unproven) treatment. Informed consent is usually required for a study to be considered ethical, including the disclosure that some test subjects will receive placebo treatments.
The ethics of placebo-controlled studies have been debated in the revision process of the Declaration of Helsinki. Of particular concern has been the difference between trials comparing inert placebos with experimental treatments, versus comparing the best available treatment with an experimental treatment; and differences between trials in the sponsor's developed countries versus the trial's targeted developing countries.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
Some suggest that existing medical treatments should be used instead of placebos, to avoid having some patients not receive medicine during the trial.<ref name="michels">Template:Cite journal</ref>
In medical practiceEdit
The practice of doctors prescribing placebos that are disguised as real medication is controversial. A chief concern is that it is deceptive and could harm the doctor–patient relationship in the long run. While some say that blanket consent, or the general consent to unspecified treatment given by patients beforehand, is ethical, others argue that patients should always obtain specific information about the name of the drug they are receiving, its side effects, and other treatment options.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> This view is shared by some on the grounds of patient autonomy.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> There are also concerns that legitimate doctors and pharmacists could open themselves up to charges of fraud or malpractice by using a placebo.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> Critics also argued that using placebos can delay the proper diagnosis and treatment of serious medical conditions.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
Despite the abovementioned issues, 60% of surveyed physicians and head nurses reported using placebos in an Israeli study, with only 5% of respondents stating that placebo use should be strictly prohibited.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> A British Medical Journal editorial said, "that a patient gets pain relief from a placebo does not imply that the pain is not real or organic in originTemplate:Spaces...the use of the placebo for 'diagnosis' of whether or not pain is real is misguided."<ref name="Spiegel">Template:Cite journal</ref> A survey in the United States of more than 10,000 physicians came to the result that while 24% of physicians would prescribe a treatment that is a placebo simply because the patient wanted treatment, 58% would not, and for the remaining 18%, it would depend on the circumstances.<ref name=kane>Doctors Struggle With Tougher-Than-Ever Dilemmas: Other Ethical Issues Author: Leslie Kane. 11/11/2010</ref>
Referring specifically to homeopathy, the House of Commons of the United Kingdom Science and Technology Committee has stated:<templatestyles src="Template:Blockquote/styles.css" />
Template:ErrorTemplate:Main other{{#if:|{{#if:|}}
— {{#if:|, in }}Template:Comma separated entries}}
{{#invoke:Check for unknown parameters|check|unknown=Template:Main other|preview=Page using Template:Blockquote with unknown parameter "_VALUE_"|ignoreblank=y| 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | author | by | char | character | cite | class | content | multiline | personquoted | publication | quote | quotesource | quotetext | sign | source | style | text | title | ts }}
In his 2008 book Bad Science, Ben Goldacre argues that instead of deceiving patients with placebos, doctors should use the placebo effect to enhance effective medicines.<ref name="bad-science">Template:Cite book</ref> Edzard Ernst has argued similarly that "As a good doctor you should be able to transmit a placebo effect through the compassion you show your patients."<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> In an opinion piece about homeopathy, Ernst argues that it is wrong to support alternative medicine on the basis that it can make patients feel better through the placebo effect.<ref name="Ernst-homeopathy"/> His concerns are that it is deceitful and that the placebo effect is unreliable.<ref name="Ernst-homeopathy">Template:Cite news</ref> Goldacre also concludes that the placebo effect does not justify alternative medicine, arguing that unscientific medicine could lead to patients not receiving prevention advice.<ref name="bad-science"/> Placebo researcher Fabrizio Benedetti also expresses concern over the potential for placebos to be used unethically, warning that there is an increase in "quackery" and that an "alternative industry that preys on the vulnerable" is developing.<ref name="Benedetti">Template:Cite journal</ref>
MechanismsEdit
The mechanism for how placebos could have effects is uncertain. From a sociocognitive perspective, intentional placebo response is attributed to the “ritual effect” that induces anticipation for transition to a better state.<ref>Template:Citation</ref> A placebo presented as a stimulant may trigger an effect on heart rhythm and blood pressure, but when administered as a depressant, the opposite effect.<ref>Template:Cite book</ref>
PsychologyEdit
In psychology, the two main hypotheses of the placebo effect are expectancy theory and classical conditioning.<ref name="podd" />
In 1985, Irving Kirsch hypothesized that placebo effects are produced by the self-fulfilling effects of response expectancies, in which the belief that one will feel different leads a person to actually feel different.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> According to this theory, the belief that one has received an active treatment can produce the subjective changes thought to be produced by the real treatment. Similarly, the appearance of effect can result from classical conditioning, wherein a placebo and an actual stimulus are used simultaneously until the placebo is associated with the effect from the actual stimulus.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> Both conditioning and expectations play a role in placebo effect,<ref name="podd">Template:Cite journal</ref> and make different kinds of contributions. Conditioning has a longer-lasting effect,<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> and can affect earlier stages of information processing.<ref name="Colloca et al 2008">Template:Cite journal</ref> Those who think a treatment will work display a stronger placebo effect than those who do not, as evidenced by a study of acupuncture.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
Additionally, motivation may contribute to the placebo effect. The active goals of an individual changes their somatic experience by altering the detection and interpretation of expectation-congruent symptoms, and by changing the behavioral strategies a person pursues.<ref name="Geers et al">Template:Cite journal</ref> Motivation may link to the meaning through which people experience illness and treatment. Such meaning is derived from the culture in which they live and which informs them about the nature of illness and how it responds to treatment.Template:Cn
Placebo analgesiaEdit
Functional imaging upon placebo analgesia suggests links to the activation, and increased functional correlation between this activation, in the anterior cingulate, prefrontal, orbitofrontal and insular cortices, nucleus accumbens, amygdala, the brainstem's periaqueductal gray matter,<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> and the spinal cord.<ref name="autogenerated2007">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
Since 1978, it has been known that placebo analgesia depends upon the release of endogenous opioids in the brain.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> Such analgesic placebos activation changes processing lower down in the brain by enhancing the descending inhibition through the periaqueductal gray on spinal nociceptive reflexes, while the expectations of anti-analgesic nocebos acts in the opposite way to block this.<ref name="autogenerated2007"/>
Functional imaging upon placebo analgesia has been summarized as showing that the placebo response is "mediated by 'top-down' processes dependent on frontal cortical areas that generate and maintain cognitive expectancies. Dopaminergic reward pathways may underlie these expectancies".<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> "Diseases lacking major 'top-down' or cortically based regulation may be less prone to placebo-related improvement".<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
Brain and bodyEdit
In conditioning, a neutral stimulus saccharin is paired in a drink with an agent that produces an unconditioned response. For example, that agent might be cyclophosphamide, which causes immunosuppression. After learning this pairing, the taste of saccharin by itself is able to cause immunosuppression, as a new conditioned response via neural top-down control.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> Such conditioning has been found to affect a diverse variety of not just basic physiological processes in the immune system but ones such as serum iron levels, oxidative DNA damage levels, and insulin secretion. Recent reviews have argued that the placebo effect is due to top-down control by the brain for immunity<ref name="Pacheco">Template:Cite journal</ref> and pain.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> Pacheco-López and colleagues have raised the possibility of "neocortical-sympathetic-immune axis providing neuroanatomical substrates that might explain the link between placebo/conditioned and placebo/expectation responses".<ref name="Pacheco"/>Template:Rp There has also been research aiming to understand underlying neurobiological mechanisms of action in pain relief, immunosuppression, Parkinson's disease and depression.<ref name=Neurobiology2005>Template:Cite journal</ref>
Dopaminergic pathways have been implicated in the placebo response in pain and depression.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
Confounding factorsEdit
Placebo-controlled studies, as well as studies of the placebo effect itself, often fail to adequately identify confounding factors.<ref name=acs/><ref name="hrob2010">Template:Cite journal</ref> False impressions of placebo effects are caused by many factors including:<ref name=acs/><ref name="Kienle & Kiene" /><ref name="hrob2010" /><ref name="podd" /><ref name=Golomb>Template:Cite journal</ref>
- Regression to the mean (natural recovery or fluctuation of symptoms)
- Additional treatments
- Response bias from subjects, including scaling bias, answers of politeness, experimental subordination, conditioned answers;
- Reporting bias from experimenters, including misjudgment and irrelevant response variables.
- Non-inert ingredients of the placebo medication having an unintended physical effect
HistoryEdit
{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}}
The word placebo was used in a medicinal context in the late 18th century to describe a "commonplace method or medicine" and in 1811 it was defined as "any medicine adapted more to please than to benefit the patient". Although this definition contained a derogatory implication<ref name="Shapiro">Template:Cite journal</ref> it did not necessarily imply that the remedy had no effect.<ref name="Kaptchuk">Template:Cite journal</ref>
It was recognized in the 18th and 19th centuries that drugs or remedies often were perceived to work best while they were still novel:<ref>Arthur K. Shapiro, Elaine Shapiro, The Powerful Placebo: From Ancient Priest to Modern Physician, 2006, Template:ISBN, chapter "The Placebo Effect in Medical History"</ref>
<templatestyles src="Template:Blockquote/styles.css" />
Template:ErrorTemplate:Main other{{#if:Gaston de Lévis quoting Michel-Philippe Bouvart in the 1780s<ref name="levis">Gaston de Lévis, Souvenirs et portraits, 1780–1789, 1813, p. 240</ref>|{{#if:|}}
— {{#if:|, in }}Template:Comma separated entries}}
{{#invoke:Check for unknown parameters|check|unknown=Template:Main other|preview=Page using Template:Blockquote with unknown parameter "_VALUE_"|ignoreblank=y| 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | author | by | char | character | cite | class | content | multiline | personquoted | publication | quote | quotesource | quotetext | sign | source | style | text | title | ts }}
Placebos have featured in medical use until well into the twentieth century.<ref name=HistoricalOverview>Template:Cite journal</ref> An influential 1955 study entitled The Powerful Placebo firmly established the idea that placebo effects were clinically important,<ref name="Hrob2001">Template:Cite journal</ref> and were a result of the brain's role in physical health. A 1997 reassessment found no evidence of any placebo effect in the source data, as the study had not accounted for regression to the mean.<ref name="Kienle & Kiene" /><ref name=Hrob2004/><ref>Stoddart, Charlotte, How placebo effect went mainstream, Knowable Magazine, June 27, 2023</ref>
Placebo-controlled studiesEdit
{{#invoke:Labelled list hatnote|labelledList|Main article|Main articles|Main page|Main pages}}
The placebo effect makes it more difficult to evaluate new treatments. Clinical trials control for this effect by including a group of subjects that receives a sham treatment. The subjects in such trials are blinded as to whether they receive the treatment or a placebo. If a person is given a placebo under one name, and they respond, they will respond in the same way on a later occasion to that placebo under that name but not if under another.<ref name="pmid18440407">Template:Cite journal</ref>
Clinical trials are often double-blinded so that the researchers also do not know which test subjects are receiving the active or placebo treatment. The placebo effect in such clinical trials is weaker than in normal therapy since the subjects are not sure whether the treatment they are receiving is active.<ref name="autogenerated2003">Template:Cite journal</ref>
See alsoEdit
- Template:Section link
- Homeopathy
- List of effects
- List of topics characterized as pseudoscience
- Nocebo
- Placebo button
- Royal Commission on Animal Magnetism
- Self-fulfilling prophecy<ref>Template:Cite book</ref>
ReferencesEdit
Further readingEdit
External linksEdit
- Program in Placebo Studies & Therapeutic Encounter (PiPS) (Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center and Harvard Medical School)
Template:Sister project Template:Sister project Template:Authority control