Template:Short description Template:Redirect Template:Use dmy dates

File:Humpbackwhale singing.webm
Humpback whales are well known for their songs. Click the arrow to play the video, which includes audio.

Whales use a variety of sounds for communication and sensation.<ref>Communication and behavior of whales, R Payne. 1983. Westview Press.</ref> The mechanisms used to produce sound vary from one family of cetaceans to another. Marine mammals, including whales, dolphins, and porpoises, are much more dependent on sound than land mammals due to the limited effectiveness of other senses in water. Sight is less effective for marine mammals because of the way particulates in the ocean scatter light. Smell is also limited, as molecules diffuse more slowly in water than in air, which makes smelling less effective. However, the speed of sound is roughly four times greater in water than in the atmosphere at sea level. As sea mammals are so dependent on hearing to communicate and feed, environmentalists and cetologists are concerned that they are being harmed by the increased ambient noise in the world's oceans caused by ships, sonar and marine seismic surveys.<ref name="Melcón2012">Template:Cite journal</ref>

The word "song" is used to describe the pattern of regular and predictable sounds made by some species of whales, notably the humpback whale. This is included with or in comparison with music, and male humpback whales have been described as "inveterate composers" of songs that are "'strikingly similar' to human musical traditions".<ref>Payne Roger, quoted in: Author(s): Susan Milius. "Music without Borders", p. 253. Source: Science News, Vol. 157, No. 16, (15 April 2000), pp. 252-254. Published by: Society for Science & the Public.</ref> This position has been complicated by more recent research, however.<ref>Template:Cite book</ref> It has been suggested that humpback songs communicate male fitness to female whales,<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> although this explanation has been challenged on various grounds.<ref>Template:Cite book</ref>

Types and purpose of vocalizationEdit

While the complex sounds of the humpback whale (and some blue whales) are believed to be primarily used in sexual selection,<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> there are simpler sounds that are created by other species of whales that have an alternative use and are used all year round.Template:Cn Whale watchers have watched mother whales lift their young towards the surface in a playful motion, while making a noise that resembles cooing in humans.<ref name=":12">Template:Cite journal</ref> This cooing-like noise made by whales seems designed to relax their young<ref name=":12" /> and is one of several distinct everyday noises whales are known to make. Unlike some fish such as sharks, a toothed whale's sense of smell is absent, causing them to rely heavily on echolocation, both for hunting prey and for navigating the ocean under darkness.Template:Cn This requires the whales to produce noise year round to ensure they are able to navigate around any obstacles they may face such as sunken ships or other animals.Template:Cn

It has also been proven that whales are extremely social creatures. The noises that are made throughout the entire year (the main sounds being whistles, clicks, and pulsed calls) are used to communicate with other members of their pod.<ref name=":4">{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref> Each sound a whale makes could mean something different. The clicking noises whales make are used for navigation.<ref name=":4" />   

The question of whether whales sometimes sing purely for aesthetic enjoyment, personal satisfaction, or 'for art's sake', is considered by some to be "an untestable question".<ref>Entomologist and ecologist Thomas Eisner called it "an untestable question in scientific terms", quoted in: Milius (2000), p. 254</ref>

Song of the humpback whaleEdit

File:HumBack2.jpg
Spectrogram of humpback whale vocalizations. Detail is shown for the first 24 seconds of the 37 second recording below.

{{#invoke:Listen|main}}

Interest in whale song was aroused by researchers Katy and Roger Payne as well as Scott McVay after the songs were brought to their attention by a Bermudian named Frank Watlington who was working for the US government at the SOFAR station listening for Russian submarines with underwater hydrophones off the coast of the island.<ref>Template:Cite book</ref> The Paynes released the best-selling Songs of the Humpback Whale in 1970, and the whale songs were quickly incorporated into human music by, among others, singer Judy Collins, as well as George Crumb, Paul Winter, and David Rothenberg.

The humpback whale produces a series of repetitious sounds at varying frequencies known as whale song. Marine biologist Philip Clapham describes the song as "probably the most complex in the animal kingdom."<ref>Template:Cite book</ref>

Male humpback whales perform these vocalizations often during the mating season, and so it was initially believed the purpose of songs is to aid mate selection.<ref name="Frankel" /> However, no evidence was found that links these songs to reprosexuality.Template:Citation needed

The songs follow a distinct hierarchical structure. The base units of the song (sometimes loosely called the "notes") are single uninterrupted emissions of sound that last up to a few seconds. These sounds vary in frequency from 20 Hz to upward of 24 kHz (the typical human range of hearing is 20 Hz to 20 kHz). The units may be frequency modulated (i.e., the pitch of the sound may go up, down, or stay the same during the note) or amplitude modulated (get louder or quieter). However, the adjustment of bandwidth on a spectrogram representation of the song reveals the essentially pulsed nature of the FM sounds.Template:Citation needed

A collection of four or six units is known as a sub-phrase, lasting perhaps ten seconds (see also phrase (music)).<ref name="Frankel" /> A collection of two sub-phrases is a phrase. A whale will typically repeat the same phrase over and over for two to four minutes. This is known as a theme. A collection of themes is known as a song.<ref name="Frankel" /> The whale song will last up to 30 or so minutes, and will be repeated over and over again over the course of hours or even days.<ref name="Frankel" /> This "Russian doll" hierarchy of sounds suggests a syntactic structure<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> that is more human-like in its complexity than other forms of animal communication like bird songs, which have only linear structure.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>

All the whales in an area sing virtually the same song at any point in time and the song is constantly and slowly evolving over time.Template:Citation needed For example, over the course of a month a particular unit that started as an upsweep (increasing in frequency) might slowly flatten to become a constant note.<ref name="Frankel" /> Another unit may get steadily louder. The pace of evolution of a whale's song also changes—some years the song may change quite rapidly, whereas in other years little variation may be recorded.<ref name="Frankel" />

Template:Wide image

File:Akhumps 128 016 0 500c.png
Humpback whale, sound spectrum and time plots

Whales occupying the same geographical areas (which can be as large as entire ocean basins) tend to sing similar songs, with only slight variations. Whales from non-overlapping regions sing entirely different songs.<ref name="Frankel" />

As the song evolves, it appears that old patterns are not revisited.<ref name="Frankel" /> An analysis of 19 years of whale songs found that while general patterns in song could be spotted, the same combination never recurred.Template:Citation needed

Humpback whales may also make stand-alone sounds that do not form part of a song, particularly during courtship rituals.<ref name="JMamm">Template:Cite journal</ref> Finally, humpbacks make a third class of sound called the feeding call.Template:Citation needed This is a long sound (5 to 10 s duration) of near constant frequency. Humpbacks generally feed cooperatively by gathering in groups, swimming underneath shoals of fish and all lunging up vertically through the fish and out of the water together. Prior to these lunges, whales make their feeding call. The exact purpose of the call is not known.

Some scientists have proposed that humpback whale songs may serve an echolocative purpose,<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> but this has been subject to disagreement.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>

Other whale soundsEdit

Humpback whales have also been found to make a range of other social sounds to communicate such as "grunts", "groans", "thwops", "snorts" and "barks".<ref>Cecilia Burke, ''A whale's varied vocabulary', Australian Geographic Template:Webarchive, AG Online. Retrieved 7 August 2010.</ref>

In 2009, researchers found that blue whale song has been deepening in its tonal frequency since the 1960s.<ref>McDonald, Mark A., Hildebrand, John A., Mesnick, Sarah. Worldwide decline in tonal frequencies of blue whale songs. Endangered Species Research, Vol. 9 No. 1 23 October 2009.</ref> While noise pollution has increased ambient ocean noise by over 12 decibels since the mid-20th century, researcher Mark McDonald indicated that higher pitches would be expected if the whales were straining to be heard.<ref>Keim, Brandon. Blue Whale Song Mystery Baffles Scientists. Wired. 2 December 2009.</ref>

Killer whales have been observed to produce long range calls that are stereotyped and high frequency travelling distances from Template:Cvt as well as short range calls that can travel distances from Template:Cvt. Short range calls are reported during social and resting periods while long range are more commonly reported during foraging and feeding.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>

Most other whales and dolphins produce sounds of varying degrees of complexity. Of particular interest is the Beluga (the "sea canary") which produces an immense variety of whistles, clicks and pulses.<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref><ref name="seaworldcom">{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref>

It was previously thought that most baleen whales make sounds at about 15–20 hertz.<ref name=":2">{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref> However, a team of marine biologists, led by Mary Ann Daher of the Woods Hole Oceanographic Institution, reported in New Scientist in December 2004 that they had been tracking a whale in the North Pacific for 12 years that was "singing" at 52 Hz. Scientists have been unable to explain this phenomenon. 52 Hz is a very low sound, it is audible through human ears as a low moaning sound.<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref> It was not expected that this whale was a new species, more so this whale indicated that a currently known species potentially has a much wider vocal range than previously thought.<ref name=":2" /> There is disagreement in the scientific community regarding the uniqueness of the whale's vocalization and whether it is a member of a hybrid whale such as the well documented blue and fin whale hybrids.

Mechanisms of sound productionEdit

Humans produce voiced sounds by passing air through the larynx. Within the larynx, when the vocal cords are brought close together, the passing air will force them to alternately close and open, separating the continuous airstream into discrete pulses of air that are heard as a vibration.<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref> This vibration is further modified by speech organs in the oral and nasal cavities, creating sounds which are used in human speech.

Cetacean sound production differs markedly from this mechanism. The precise mechanism differs in the two suborders of cetaceans: the Odontoceti (toothed whales, including dolphins) and the Mysticeti (baleen whales, including the largest whales such as the blue whale).

Odontocete whalesEdit

File:Delfinekko.gif
Process in a dolphin echolocation: in green the sounds generated by the dolphin, in red from the fish.
File:Dolphin head.svg
Idealized dolphin head showing the regions involved in sound production. This image was redrawn from Cranford (2000).

Odontocetes produce rapid bursts of high-frequency clicks that are thought to be primarily for echolocation. Specialized organs in an odontocete produce collections of clicks and buzzes at frequencies from 0.2 to 150 kHz to obtain sonic information about its environment. Lower frequencies are used for distance echolocation, due to the fact that shorter wavelengths do not travel as far as longer wavelengths underwater. Higher frequencies are more effective at shorter distances, and can reveal more detailed information about a target. Echoes from clicks convey not only the distance to the target, but also the size, shape, speed, and vector of its movement. Additionally, echolocation allows the odontocete to easily discern the difference between objects that are different in material composition, even if visually identical, by their different densities. Individuals also appear to be able to isolate their own echoes during pod feeding activity without interference from other pod members' echolocations.<ref name="dolphins.org">{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref>

Whistles are used for communication. Four- to six-month-old calves develop unique sounds that they use most frequently throughout their lives. Such "signature whistles" are distinctive to the individual and may serve as a form of identification among other odontocetes.<ref name="dolphins.org"/> Though a large pod of dolphins will produce a wide range of different noises, very little is known about the meaning of the sound. Frankel quotes one researcher who says listening to a school of odontocetes is like listening to a group of children at a school playground.<ref name=Frankel>Frankel, Adam S. "Sound production", Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals, 1998, pp. 1126–1137. Template:ISBN.</ref>

The multiple sounds odontocetes make are produced by passing air through a structure in the head called the phonic lips.<ref name=":0">Template:Cite journal</ref> Biologically the structure is homologous to an upper lip located in the nasal cavity, but mechanistically the phonic lips act similarly to human vocal "cords" (vocal folds), which in humans are located in the larynx. As the air passes through this narrow passage, the phonic lip membranes are sucked together, causing the surrounding tissue to vibrate. These vibrations can, as with the vibrations in the human larynx, be consciously controlled with great sensitivity.<ref name=":0" /> The vibrations pass through the tissue of the head to the melon, which shapes and directs the sound into a beam of sound useful in echolocation. Every toothed whale except the sperm whale has two sets of phonic lips and is thus capable of making two sounds independently.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> Once the air has passed the phonic lips it enters the vestibular sac. From there, the air may be recycled back into the lower part of the nasal complex, ready to be used for sound creation again, or passed out through the blowhole.<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref>

The French name for phonic lips, museau de singe, translates literally as "monkey's muzzle", which the phonic lip structure is supposed to resemble in sperm whales.<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref> New cranial analysis using computed axial and single photon emission computed tomography scans in 2004 showed, at least in the case of bottlenose dolphins, that air might be supplied to the nasal complex from the lungs, enabling the sound creation process to continue for as long as the dolphin can add air from the lungs.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>

Sperm whaleEdit

Template:Further The sperm whale's vocalizations are all based on clicking, described in four types: the usual echolocation, creaks, codas, and slow clicks.<ref name="Whitehead book">Template:Cite book</ref> The most distinctive vocalizations are codas, which are short rhythmic sequences of clicks, mostly numbering 3–12 clicks, in stereotyped patterns.<ref name="Whitehead-2024">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="Whitehead book"/> They are the result of vocal learning within a stable social group.<ref name="Gero-2016">Template:Cite journal</ref> Some codas express clan identity, and denote different patterns of travel, foraging, and socializing or avoidance among clans.<ref name="Safina-2020-pp16–19">Template:Cite book</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> As “arbitrary traits that function as reliable indicators of cultural group membership,” clan identity codas act as symbolic markers that modulate interactions between individuals.<ref name="Hersh-2022">Template:Cite journal</ref>

Individual identity in sperm whale vocalizations is an ongoing scientific issue, however. A distinction needs to be made between cues and signals. Human acoustic tools can distinguish individual whales by analyzing micro-characteristics of their vocalizations, and the whales can probably do the same. This does not prove that the whales deliberately use some vocalizations to signal individual identity in the manner of the signature whistles that bottlenose dolphins use as individual labels.<ref name="Gero-2016"/><ref name="Hersh-2022"/>

Mysticete whalesEdit

Mysticetes do not have phonic lip structure. Instead, they have a larynx that appears to play a role in sound production, as it has vocal folds (vocal "cord") homologs in the U-shaped fold supported by arytenoid cartilages.<ref name=":1" /> Whales do not have to exhale in order to produce sound, as they capture the air in a laryngeal sac. It is likely that they recycle air from this sac back to the lungs for the next vocalization.<ref name=":1">Template:Cite journal</ref> They do not have bony cranial sinuses, but there is a pterygoid air sac. Its role in sound production is unclear (perhaps resonance?), but most likely it is for hearing, as it appears to preserve an airspace at depth around the ear ossicles.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>

Vocal plasticity and acoustic behaviorEdit

There are at least nine separate blue whale acoustic populations worldwide.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> Over the last 50 years blue whales have changed the way they are singing. Calls are progressively getting lower in frequency. For example, the Australian pygmy blue whales are decreasing their mean call frequency rate at approximately 0.35 Hz/year.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>

The migration patterns of blue whales remain unclear. Some populations appear to be resident in habitats of year-round high productivity in some years,<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> while others undertake long migrations to high-latitude feeding grounds, but the extent of migrations and the components of the populations that undertake them are poorly known.<ref name = "IUCNblue">Template:Cite iucn</ref>

Sound levelsEdit

The frequency of baleen whale sounds ranges from 10 Hz to 31 kHz.<ref name=RGMT>Template:Cite book</ref> A list of typical levels is shown in the table below.

Source Broadband source level (dB re 1 <math>\mu</math>Pa at 1m)<ref name=KupermanRoux>Template:Cite book</ref>
Fin whale moans 155–186
Blue whale moans 155–188
Gray whale moans 142–185
Bowhead whale tonals, moans and song 128–189

Human interactionEdit

Researchers use hydrophones (often adapted from their original military use in tracking submarines) to ascertain the exact location of the origin of whale noises.Template:Citation needed Their methods also allow them to detect how far through an ocean a sound travels.Template:Citation needed Research by Dr. Christopher Clark of Cornell University conducted using military data showed that whale noises travel for thousands of kilometres.<ref name="BBC">Template:Cite news</ref> As well as providing information about song production, the data allows researchers to follow the migratory path of whales throughout the "singing" (mating) season. An important finding is that whales, in a process called the Lombard effect, adjust their song to compensate for background noise pollution.<ref name="Scheifele">Template:Cite journal</ref>

File:Example of Blue Whales' D calls in presence of MFA sonar - Melcón et al. 2012.png
Blue whales stop producing foraging D calls once a mid-frequency sonar is activated, even though the sonar frequency range (1–8 kHz) far exceeds their sound production range (25–100 Hz).<ref name="Melcón2012" />

Moreover, there is evidence that blue whales stop producing foraging D calls once a mid-frequency sonar is activated, even though the sonar frequency range (1–8 kHz) far exceeds their sound production range (25–100 Hz).<ref name="Melcón2012" />

File:The Sounds of Earth - GPN-2000-001976.jpg
Voyager Golden Records carried whale songs into outer space with other sounds representing planet Earth.

Research indicates that ambient noise from boats is doubling with each decade,<ref name="BBC" /> reducing the range at which whale sounds can be heard. Prior to the introduction of large-scale shipping, whale sounds may have traveled from one side of an ocean to the other.<ref name="BBC" /> Environmentalists fear that such boat activity is putting undue stress on the animals as well as making it difficult to find a mate.<ref name="BBC" />

In the past decade, many effective automated methods, such as signal processing, data mining, and machine learning techniques have been developed to detect and classify whale vocalizations.<ref>M. Pourhomayoun, P. Dugan, M. Popescu, and C. Clark, "Bioacoustic Signal Classification Based on Continuous Region Features, Grid Masking Features and Artificial Neural Network", International Conference on Machine Learning (ICML), 2013.</ref><ref>7. M. Popescu, P. Dugan, M. Pourhomayoun, and C. Clark, "Periodic Pulse Train Signal Detection and Classification using Spectrogram Intensity Binarization and Energy Projection", International Conference on Machine Learning (ICML), 2013.</ref>

HistoryEdit

Whaling Captain Wm. H. Kelly was the first person known to recognize whale singing for what it was, while on the brig Eliza in the Sea of Japan in 1881.<ref name="Aldrich (Outing) 1889">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="Aldrich 1889">Template:Cite book</ref>

After William E. Schevill became an Associate in Physical Oceanography at Woods Hole Oceanographic Institution (WHOI) in Massachusetts in 1943, his first work was under US Naval auspices investigating echolocation of U-boats.<ref name="onlinelibrary.wiley.com">Template:Cite journal</ref> As he later wrote in 1962: "During World War II many people on both sides listened to underwater sounds for military reasons. Not only the wanted sounds (those made by enemy ships), but a bewildering variety of others were heard. Most of these were ascribed to animals living in the sea, usually as 'fish noises' ... Some were ascribed to whales, in part correctly, but without identification of the kind of whale; most military listeners were not biologists, and in any case the traditional naval sonar room is woefully deficient in windows."<ref>SCHEVILL, W. E., 1962 Whale music. Oceanus 9 (2): 2–13.</ref> Schevill produced the first recordings of underwater whale sounds and extrapolated their purpose from these recordings. His groundbreaking work produced over fifty papers on whale phonation and thus provided the framework for “literally hundreds of scientific studies produced by other workers from the 1960s until the present day."<ref name="onlinelibrary.wiley.com"/> However, it is worthy of note that his wife Barbara Lawrence, Curator of Mammals at the Harvard Museum of Comparative Zoology (MCZ), often co-wrote these documents with him.<ref name="euppublishing.com">Template:Cite journal</ref>

William E. Schevill's study of whales also at one point harked back to the U.S. Naval operations that first set him down this path. As noted upon his death by the Society for the Bibliography of Natural History, "Bill helped defuse a tense moment between the USA and Soviet Union during the Cold War. The US military suspected that low frequency blips were being used by the Soviets to locate American submarines, whereas Bill showed these were produced by fin whales (Balaenoptera physalus) hunting prey."<ref name="euppublishing.com"/>

MediaEdit

Template:See also

{{#invoke:Listen|main}}

File:Beluga vocalizations.ogg
Beluga whale vocalizations published by NOAA.

Selected discographyEdit

  • Songs of the Humpback Whale (SWR 118) was originally released in 1970 by CRM Records from recordings made by Roger Payne, Frank Watlington, and others. The LP was later re-released by Capitol Records, published in a flexible format in the National Geographic Society magazine, Volume 155, Number 1, in January 1979, re-released by Living Music/Windham Hill/BMG Records on CD in 1992, and remastered on CD by BGO-Beat Goes On in 2001.
  • Deep Voices: The Second Whale Record (Capitol/EMI Records 0777 7 11598 1 0) was released on LP in 1977 from additional recordings made by Roger Payne, and re-released on CD in 1995 by Living Music/Windham Hill/BMG Records. It includes recordings of humpbacks, blues, and rights.
  • Northern Whales (MGE 19) was released by Music Gallery Editions from recordings made by Pierre Ouellet, John Ford, and others affiliated with Interspecies Music and Communication Research. It includes recordings of belugas, narwhals, orca, and bearded seals.
  • Sounds of the Earth: Humpback Whales (Oreade Music) was released on CD in 1999.
  • Rapture of the Deep: Humpback Whale Singing (Compass Recordings) was released on CD in 2001.
  • Songlines: Songs of the East Australian Humpback whales. was released in 2009.

See alsoEdit

Template:Stack

ReferencesEdit

Template:Reflist

General referencesEdit

Template:Refbegin

  • Lone whale's song remains a mystery, New Scientist, issue number 2477, 11 December 2004
  • Template:Cite journal
  • Helweg, D.A., Frankel, A.S., Mobley Jr, J.R. and Herman, L.M., "Humpback whale song: our current understanding," in Marine Mammal Sensory Systems, J. A. Thomas, R. A. Kastelein, and A. Y. Supin, Eds. New York: Plenum, 1992, pp. 459–483.
  • In search of impulse sound sources in odontocetes by Ted Cranford in Hearing by whales and dolphins (W. Lu, A. Popper and R. Fays eds.). Springer-Verlag (2000).
  • Progressive changes in the songs of humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae): a detailed analysis of two seasons in Hawaii by K.B.Payne, P. Tyack and R.S. Payne in Communication and behavior of whales. Westview Press (1983)

Template:Refend

External linksEdit

Template:Animal communication Template:Hydroacoustics Template:Zoomusicology