Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Common raven
(section)
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
=== Intelligence === {{See also|Bird intelligence|Corvidae#Intelligence}} The brain of the common raven is among the largest of any bird species. Specifically, their [[avian pallium|hyperpallium]] is large for a bird. They display ability in problem-solving, as well as other [[cognitive]] processes such as [[imitation]] and [[Insight phenomenology|insight]].<ref name=birdinblack/> [[File:Old NIKE Missile radar dome with ravens.JPG|thumb|Dilapidated [[Project Nike|Nike Missile]] radar dome in Alaska with an evening roost]] Linguist [[Derek Bickerton]], building on the work of biologist [[Bernd Heinrich]], has argued that ravens are one of only four known animals (the others being [[bee]]s, [[ant]]s, and humans) who have demonstrated [[Displacement (linguistics)|displacement]], the capacity to communicate about objects or events that are distant in space or time. Subadult ravens roost together at night, but usually forage alone during the day. However, when one discovers a large carcass guarded by a pair of adult ravens, the unmated raven will return to the roost and communicate the find. The following day, a flock of unmated ravens will fly to the carcass and chase off the adults. Bickerton argues that the advent of linguistic displacement was perhaps the most important event in the evolution of human language, and that ravens are the only other [[vertebrate]] to share this with humans.<ref>Bickerton, Derek (2009). ''Adam's Tongue''. Hill and Wang. {{ISBN|978-0-8090-2281-6}}</ref> One experiment designed to evaluate insight and [[problem-solving]] ability involved a piece of meat attached to a string hanging from a perch. To reach the food, the bird needed to stand on the perch, pull the string up a little at a time, and step on the loops to gradually shorten the string. Four of five common ravens eventually succeeded, and "the transition from no success (ignoring the food or merely yanking at the string) to constant reliable access (pulling up the meat) occurred with no demonstrable [[trial-and-error]] learning." This supports the hypothesis that common ravens are 'inventors', implying that they can solve problems. Many of the feats of common ravens were formerly argued to be stereotyped innate behaviour, but it now has been established that their aptitudes for solving problems individually and learning from each other reflect a flexible capacity for intelligent insight unusual among non-human animals.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Heinrich |first=Bernd |year=1995 |title=An Experimental Investigation of Insight in Common Ravens (''Corvus corax'')|jstor=4089030|journal=The Auk|volume= 112|issue=4 |pages=994β1003 |url=http://sora.unm.edu/sites/default/files/journals/auk/v112n04/p0994-p1003.pdf |access-date= 2007-05-16|doi=10.2307/4089030}}</ref> Another experiment showed that some common ravens could intentionally deceive their conspecifics.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Bugnyar|first1=Thomas|last2=Kotrschal|first2=Kurt|title=Leading a conspecific away from food in ravens (''Corvus corax'')?|journal=Animal Cognition|date=2004|volume=7|issue=2|pages=69β76|url=http://klf.univie.ac.at/fileadmin/user_upload/p_klf/Bugnyar_and_Kotrschal2004.pdf|doi=10.1007/s10071-003-0189-4|pmid=15069605|s2cid=6590368|url-status=dead|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20150928155529/http://klf.univie.ac.at/fileadmin/user_upload/p_klf/Bugnyar_and_Kotrschal2004.pdf|archive-date=2015-09-28}}</ref> A study published in 2011 found that ravens can recognise when they are given an unfair trade during reciprocal interactions with conspecifics or humans, retaining memory of the interaction for a prolonged period of time. Birds that were given a fair trade by experimenters were found to prefer interacting with these experimenters compared to those that did not.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=MΓΌller|first1=J. J. A.|last2=Massen|first2=J. J. M.|last3=Bugnyar|first3=T.|last4=Osvath|first4=M.|date=2017-06-01|title=Ravens remember the nature of a single reciprocal interaction sequence over 2 days and even after a month|url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0003347217301161|journal=Animal Behaviour|language=en|volume=128|pages=69β78|doi=10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.04.004|s2cid=27527201 |issn=0003-3472|hdl=1887/76850|hdl-access=free}}</ref> Furthermore, ravens in the wild have also been observed to stop cooperating with other ravens if they observe them cheating during group tasks.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Massen|first1=Jorg J. M.|last2=Ritter|first2=Caroline|last3=Bugnyar|first3=Thomas|date=2015-10-07|title=Tolerance and reward equity predict cooperation in ravens (Corvus corax)|journal=Scientific Reports|language=en|volume=5|issue=1|pages=15021|doi=10.1038/srep15021|pmid=26442633 |pmc=4595729 |bibcode=2015NatSR...515021M |issn=2045-2322}}</ref> Common ravens have been observed calling [[wolf|wolves]] to the site of dead animals. The wolves open the carcass, leaving the scraps more accessible to the birds.<ref name=birdinblack>{{cite web|url=https://www.pbs.org/wnet/nature/ravens/ravens.html |title=PBS Nature: The Bird in Black |access-date=2007-05-07 |publisher=[[PBS]] |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20080717192617/http://www.pbs.org/wnet/nature/ravens/ravens.html |archive-date=July 17, 2008 }}</ref> They watch where other common ravens bury their food and remember the locations of each other's food caches, so they can steal from them. This type of theft occurs so regularly that common ravens will fly extra distances from a food source to find better hiding places for food.<ref>{{cite web|url= http://www.gi.alaska.edu/ScienceForum/ASF14/1426.html|title= The Raven's Game of Hide and Seek|access-date= 2007-05-07|last= Rozell|first= Ned|work= Alaska Science Forum|publisher= Geophysical Institute, University of Alaska Fairbanks|archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20070611185753/http://gi.alaska.edu/ScienceForum/ASF14/1426.html|archive-date= 2007-06-11|url-status= dead}}</ref> They have also been observed pretending to make a cache without actually depositing the food, presumably to confuse onlookers.<ref>[[#Marzluff|Marzluff and Angell]], p. 230</ref> Common ravens are known to steal and cache shiny objects such as pebbles, pieces of metal, and golf balls. One theory is that they hoard shiny objects to impress other ravens.<ref>[[#Marzluff|Marzluff and Angell]], p. 232</ref> Other research indicates that juveniles are deeply curious about all new things, and that common ravens retain an attraction to bright, round objects based on their similarity to bird eggs. Mature birds lose their intense interest in the unusual, and become highly [[neophobia|neophobic]].<ref>{{cite journal|doi=10.1007/s10211-002-0061-6|author1=Kijne, M. |author2=Kotrschal, K |year=2002|title=Neophobia affects choice of food-item size in group-foraging common ravens (''Corvus corax'')|journal=Acta Ethologica|issue=1|pages= 13β18|volume=5|s2cid=39984855 }}</ref> The first large-scale assessment of ravens' cognitive abilities suggests that, by four months of age, ravens do about as well as adult [[chimpanzee]]s and [[orangutan]]s on tests of [[causal reasoning]], [[Social learning in animals|social learning]], [[theory of mind]], etc.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Pika |first1=Simone |last2=Sima |first2=Miriam Jennifer |last3=Blum |first3=Christian R. |last4=Herrmann |first4=Esther |last5=Mundry |first5=Roger |date=2020-12-10 |title=Ravens parallel great apes in physical and social cognitive skills |journal=Scientific Reports |language=en |volume=10 |issue=1 |pages=20617 |doi=10.1038/s41598-020-77060-8 |pmid=33303790 |pmc=7728792 |bibcode=2020NatSR..1020617P |s2cid=228099688 |issn=2045-2322}}</ref>
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)