Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Second-language acquisition
(section)
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
=== Sociocultural factors === From the early days of the discipline, researchers have also acknowledged that social aspects play an important role.{{sfn|Ellis|1997|p=37}} There have been many different approaches to the sociolinguistic study of second-language acquisition.{{sfn|Ellis|2008|pp=280β281}} Common to each of these approaches, however, is a rejection of language as a purely psychological phenomenon; instead, sociolinguistic research views the social context in which language is learned as essential for a proper understanding of the acquisition process.{{sfn|Gass|Selinker|2008|pp=280β281}} Ellis identifies three types of social structures that affect the acquisition of second languages: sociolinguistic settings, specific social factors, and situational factors.{{sfn|Ellis|2008|p=281}} Sociolinguistic setting refers to the role of the second language in society, such as whether it is spoken by a majority or a minority of the population, whether its use is widespread or restricted to a few functional roles, or whether the society is predominantly bilingual or monolingual.{{sfn|Siegel|2003|p=178}} Ellis also includes the distinction of whether the second language is learned in a natural or an educational setting.{{sfn|Ellis|2008|p=288}} Specific social factors that can affect second-language acquisition include age, gender, social class, and ethnic identity, with ethnic identity being the one that has received most research attention.{{sfn|Ellis|2008|p=323}} Situational factors are those that vary between each social interaction. For example, a learner may use more polite language when talking to someone of higher social status, but more informal language when talking with friends.{{sfn|Gass|Selinker|2008|pp=268β269}} A learner's sense of connection to their in-group, as well as to the community of the target language emphasizes the influence of the sociolinguistic setting, as well as social factors within the second-language acquisition process. [[Social identity theory|Social Identity Theory]] argues that an important factor for second language acquisition is the learner's perceived identity to the community of the language being learned, as well as how the community of the target language perceives the learner.{{sfn|Loewen|Reinders|2011}} Whether or not a learner feels a sense of connection to the community or culture of the target language helps determine their social distance from the target culture. A smaller social distance is likely to encourage learners to acquire the second language, as their investment in the learning process is greater. Conversely, a greater social distance discourages attempts to acquire the target language. However, negative views not only come from the learner, but the community of the target language might feel greater social distance from the learner, limiting the learner's ability to learn the language.{{sfn|Loewen|Reinders|2011}} Whether or not bilingualism is valued by the culture or community of the learner is an important indicator of the motivation to learn a language.{{sfn|Vega|2008|pp=185β198}} There have been several models developed to explain social effects on language acquisition. Schumann's [[acculturation model]] proposes that learners' rate of development and ultimate level of language achievement is a function of the "social distance" and the "psychological distance" between learners and the second-language community. In Schumann's model, the social factors are most important, but the degree to which learners are comfortable with learning the second language also plays a role.{{sfn|Ellis|2008|pp=326β327}} Another sociolinguistic model is Gardner's [[socio-educational model]], which was designed to explain classroom language acquisition. Gardner's model focuses on the emotional aspects of SLA, arguing that positive motivation contributes to an individual's willingness to learn L2; furthermore, the goal of an individual to learn an L2 is based on the idea that the individual has a desire to be part of a culture, in other words, part of a (the targeted language) mono-linguistic community. Factors, such as [[Motivation in second-language learning|''integrativeness'']] and ''attitudes towards the learning situation'' drive motivation. The outcome of positive motivation is not only linguistic but non-linguistic, such that the learner has met the desired goal. Although there are many critics of Gardner's model, nonetheless many of these critics have been influenced by the merits that his model holds.<ref>{{Cite journal|title = A Critical Review on the Socio-educational Model of SLA|journal = Theory and Practice in Language Studies|date = 2015-03-24|issn = 1799-2591|pages = 605β612|volume = 5|issue = 3|doi = 10.17507/tpls.0503.21|first1 = Masumeh|last1 = Taie|first2 = Asghar|last2 = Afshari|doi-access = free|hdl = 10818/30256|hdl-access = free}}</ref>{{sfn|Ellis|2008|p=330}} The [[inter-group model]] proposes "ethnolinguistic vitality" as a key construct for second-language acquisition.{{sfn|Ellis|2008|p=332}} [[Language socialization]] is an approach with the premise that "linguistic and cultural knowledge are ''constructed'' through each other",{{sfn|Watson-Gegeo|Nielsen|2003|p=157}} and saw increased attention after the year 2000.{{sfn|Ellis|2008|p=334}} Finally, Norton's theory of [[social identity (linguistics)|social identity]] is an attempt to codify the relationship between power, identity, and language acquisition.{{sfn|Ellis|2008|p=336}} A unique approach to SLA is sociocultural theory. It was originally developed by [[Lev Vygotsky]] and his followers.{{sfn|VanPatten|Benati|2010|pp=151β152}} Central to Vygotsky's theory is the concept of a [[zone of proximal development]] (ZPD). The ZPD notion states that social interaction with more advanced target language users allows one to learn a language at a higher level than if they were to learn a language independently.<ref>{{Cite book|title=How Languages are Learned|last1=Lightbown|first1=Patsy|last2=Spada|first2=Nina|publisher=Oxford University Press|year=2011|isbn=978-0-19-442224-6}}</ref> Sociocultural theory has a fundamentally different set of assumptions to approaches to second-language acquisition based on the computational model.{{sfn|Ellis|2008|pp=517β518}} Furthermore, although it is closely affiliated with other social approaches, it is a theory of mind and not of general social explanation of language acquisition. According to Ellis, "It is important to recognize... that this paradigm, despite the label 'sociocultural' does not seek to explain how learners acquire the cultural values of the L2 but rather how knowledge of an L2 is internalized through experiences of a sociocultural nature."{{sfn|Ellis|2008|pp=517β518}}
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)