Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Flashbulb memory
(section)
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
===Gender=== {{Research paper|section|date=May 2023}} Some studies conducted in this area of research yielded findings indicating that women are able to produce more vivid details of events and recall autobiographical events elicited by Senate hearings than men. One such study had participants fill out questionnaires about flashbulb memories and recollections of autobiographical events pertaining to [[Clarence Thomas Supreme Court nomination|the Senate hearings]] that confirmed [[Clarence Thomas]] as a Supreme Court Justice (Morse, 1993).<ref>{{Cite journal | last1 = Morse | first1 = Claire K. | last2 = Woodward | first2 = Elizabeth M. | last3 = Zweigenhaft | first3 = R. L. | doi = 10.1080/00224545.1993.9712169 | title = Gender Differences in Flashbulb Memories Elicited by the Clarence Thomas Hearings | journal = The Journal of Social Psychology | volume = 133 | issue = 4 | pages = 453β458 |date=August 1993 | pmid = 8231123 }}</ref> The study found that half of the individuals reported vivid memory images associated with the hearings. 64% of women reported images as opposed to 33% men. 77% of women reported having had stimulated recall of an autobiographical event, while only 27% of men indicated having experienced such recall. Women were more likely than men to report additional imagery (24% of women and 6% of men). Women were more likely than men to report vivid image memories and recall of autobiographical events elicited by the hearings, but they did not differ from men in the ratings of these memories. There was also no difference in the average amount of time spent consuming media on the hearing. A large body of research was conducted into events taking place during the 9/11 terrorist attacks, although it was not specifically researching gender differences. In one study researchers had participants answer questions to establish "consistent flashbulb memory," which consists of details about where the participants were at the time of the attacks, what they were doing, etc. In 2002 it was found that 49% of women and 47% of men fulfilled these requirements. In 2003, this dropped to 46% of women and 44% of men (Conway, 2009).<ref>{{Cite journal | last1 = Conway | first1 = Andrew R. A. | last2 = Skitka | first2 = Linda J. | last3 = Hemmerich | first3 = Joshua A. | last4 = Kershaw | first4 = Trina C. | title = Flashbulb memory for 11 September 2001 | doi = 10.1002/acp.1497 | journal = Applied Cognitive Psychology | volume = 23 | issue = 5 | pages = 605β623 | date = July 2009 | url = https://www.princeton.edu/~aconway/pdf/flashbulb_2008.pdf | access-date = 15 February 2013 | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20120425111626/http://www.princeton.edu/~aconway/pdf/flashbulb_2008.pdf | archive-date = 25 April 2012 | url-status = dead }}</ref> Women seemed more likely to have a more consistent memory for the event than men in this study. A longer time since the incident decreases the consistency of the memory. However, a study aimed at finding whether a series of terrorist attacks with common features elicit flashbulb memories found a different pattern of gender effects. Men rated the distinctiveness of their flashbulb-producing event higher than women did. Additionally, men had memories with more detail than women. Women however, reported higher rates of emotional reactivity.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Edery-Halpern|first1=Galit|last2=Nachson|first2=Israel|date=March 2004|title=Distinctiveness in flashbulb memory: Comparative analysis of five terrorist attacks|journal=Memory|volume=12|issue=2|pages=147β157|doi=10.1080/09658210244000432|pmid=15250180|s2cid=31338900|issn=0965-8211}}</ref> Biological reasons for gender variances in flashbulb memory may be explained by amygdala asymmetry. The [[amygdala]] is a part of the [[limbic system]], and is linked with memory and emotion. Memory is enhanced by emotion, and studies have shown that people are more likely to remember a negative event than a neutral or positive one. Investigations into the amygdala revealed "people who showed strong amygdala activation in response to a set of positive or negative stimuli (relative to other study participants) also showed superior memory for those stimuli (relative to other study participants)".<ref name="Kensinger2007">{{Cite journal | last1 = Kensinger | first1 = Elizabeth A. | title = Negative Emotion Enhances Memory Accuracy: Behavioral and Neuroimaging Evidence | doi = 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00506.x | journal = Current Directions in Psychological Science | volume = 16 | issue = 4 | pages = 213β218 |date=August 2007 | s2cid = 16885166 | url = https://www2.bc.edu/~kensinel/Kensinger_CD07.pdf <!-- alternate URL: https://www2.bc.edu/elizabeth-kensinger/Kensinger_CD07.pdf --> | url-access = subscription }}</ref> This may explain why flashbulb memory typically involves traumatic events. When viewing emotional content, research has shown that men enhance their memory by activating their right amygdala while women activate the left side.<ref name="Kensinger2007" /> Although it is still unclear how lateralization affects memory, there may be a more effective relationship between activation of the left amygdala and memory than activation of right and memory.{{Medical citation needed|date=November 2021}} Generally speaking, studies testing differences between genders on [[episodic memory]] tasks revealed that "women consistently outperform men on tasks that require remembering items that are verbal in nature or can be verbally labeled" (Herlitz, 2008).<ref name="Herlitz 2008 52β56">{{Cite journal | last1 = Herlitz | first1 = Agneta | last2 = Rehnman | first2 = Jenny | doi = 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00547.x | title = Sex Differences in Episodic Memory | journal = Current Directions in Psychological Science | volume = 17 | issue = 1 | pages = 52β56 | date = February 2008 | s2cid = 145107751 | url = http://www.augustana.edu/users/psmccallum/ps100%20Fall%202009/sex%20differences%20in%20episodic%20memory.pdf | access-date = 2013-02-18 | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20120227205236/http://www.augustana.edu/users/psmccallum/ps100%20Fall%202009/sex%20differences%20in%20episodic%20memory.pdf | archive-date = 2012-02-27 | url-status = dead }}</ref> It seems that women also "excel on tasks requiring little or no verbal processing, such as recognition of unfamiliar odors or faces" (Herlitz, 2008).<ref name="Herlitz 2008 52β56" /> Men only seem to excel in memory tasks that require [[visuospatial]] processing. Gender differences are also very apparent in research on autobiographical memory. To sum up these gender differences, most literature on memory indicates that:<ref name=":0">{{Cite journal |last1 = Bloise |first1 = Susan M. |last2 = Johnson |first2 = Marcia K. |doi = 10.1080/09658210701204456 |title = Memory for emotional and neutral information: Gender and individual differences in emotional sensitivity |journal = Memory |volume = 15 |issue = 2 |pages = 192β204 |date = February 2007 |pmid = 17534112 |s2cid = 352374 |url = http://memlab0.eng.yale.edu/PDFs/2007_Bloise_Johnson_Memory.pdf |access-date = 2013-02-18 |archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20130319071446/http://memlab0.eng.yale.edu/PDFs/2007_Bloise_Johnson_Memory.pdf |archive-date = 2013-03-19 |url-status = dead }}</ref> {{blockquote|Women use a greater quantity and variety of emotion words than men when describing their past experiences ... Women include not only a greater number of references to their own emotional states but also a greater number of references to the emotional states of others. In addition, when asked to recall emotional life experiences, women recall more memories of both positive and negative personal experiences than men.|author=Bloise & Johnson, 2007}} Overall women seem to have better memory performance than men in both emotional and non-emotional events.<ref name=":0" /> There are many problems with assessing gender differences found in the research into this topic. The clearest is that it is heavily reliant on self-reporting of events. Inaccuracy of findings could result from biased questions or participants misremembering. There is no way to completely verify the accuracy of accounts given by the subjects in a study. Additionally, there are many indications that [[eye-witness]] memory can often be fallible. Emotion does not seem to improve memory performance in a situation that involves weapons. Eyewitnesses remember fewer details about perpetrators if a weapon is involved in an event (Pickel, 2009).<ref>{{Cite journal | last1 = Pickel | first1 = Kerri L. | title = The weapon focus effect on memory for female versus male perpetrators | doi = 10.1080/09658210903029412 | journal = Memory | volume = 17 | issue = 6 | pages = 664β678 |date=August 2009 | pmid = 19536689 | s2cid = 10272699 }}</ref> Accuracy in these situations is compromised by a phenomenon known as the [[weapon focus|weapon focus effect]]. Further complicating matters is the time frame in which people are surveyed in relation to the event as many studies survey people well after the events. Thus, there is a validity issue with much of the research into flashbulb memory in general, as well as any apparent gender differences.
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)