Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Inalienable possession
(section)
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
==Cross-linguistic properties== Although there are different methods of marking inalienability, inalienable possession constructions usually involve the following features:<ref name=Heine2/> * The distinction is confined to attributive possession. * Alienable possession requires more [[phonological]] or [[morphology (linguistics)|morphological]] features than inalienable possession. *Inalienable possession involves a tighter structural bond between the possessor and the possessee. * Possessive markers on inalienable nouns are [[etymologically]] older{{efn|For example, in the [[Native American language]] [[Kumeyaay people#Language|Diegueño]], the alienable possessive marker ('''''?-ə'''n<sup>y</sup>'') appears to have originated from the inalienable possessive marker ('''''?-ə'''''), which suggests the latter to be older.<ref name="space and time">{{cite book|last1=Nichols|first1=Johanna|title=Linguistic Diversity in Space and Time|date=1992|publisher=University of Chicago Press|location=Chicago|pages=116–123|edition=ACLS Humanities E-Book}}</ref>}} * Inalienable nouns include kinship terms and/or body parts. * Inalienable nouns form a [[closed class]], but alienable nouns form an [[open class (linguistics)|open class]]. (Heine 1997: 85-86 (1-6)) ===Restricted to attributive possession=== [[File:Attributive possession DP.jpg|thumb|170px|Attribution possession: the possessor (Ron) and the possessee (dog) form a [[phrase]].]] [[File:Predicative possession 1.jpg|thumb|right|170px|Predicative possession: the possessor (Ron) and the possessee (dog) form not a phrase but instead a [[clause]].]] Alienability can be expressed only in attributive possession constructions, not in predicative possession.<ref name=Heine2/> Attributive possession is a type of possession in which the possessor and possessee form a [[phrase]]. That contrasts to predicative possession constructions in which the possessor and possessee are part of a [[clause]], and the verb affirms the possessive relationship.<ref name="Herslund and Baron">{{cite book|last1=Herslund|first1=Michael|last2=Baron|first2=Irène|title=Dimensions of Possession|date=2001|publisher=John Benjamins Publishing|location=Amsterdam|isbn=978-9027229519|pages=1–15|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=lcCKtWG3wsAC&q=Dimensions+of+Possession&pg=PA1|access-date=11 December 2014}}</ref> The examples in (22) express the same alienable relationship between possessor and possessee but illustrate the difference between attributive and predicative possession: {| |- | '''Attributive possession''' (22) a. Ron's dog '''Predicative possession''' b. Ron has a dog c. The dog is Ron's (Heine 1997: 87 (2)) |} ===Requires fewer morphological features=== If a language has separate alienable and inalienable possession constructions, and one of the constructions is overtly marked and the other is "zero-marked", the marked form tends to be alienable possession. Inalienable possession is indicated by the absence of the overt marker.<ref name=Haspelmath>{{cite web|last1=Haspelmath|first1=Martin|title=Alienable vs. inalienable possessive constructions|url=http://www.eva.mpg.de/lingua/conference/08_springschool/pdf/course_materials/Haspelmath_Possessives.pdf|website=Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology|publisher=Leipzig Spring School on Linguistic Diversity|access-date=9 November 2014}}</ref> An example is the [[Inalienable possession#No overt possessive markers|data from Dâw]]. One typological study showed that in 78% of South American languages that distinguish between inalienable and alienable possession, inalienable possession was associated with fewer [[morphology (linguistics)|morphological]] markers than was its alienable counterpart. By contrast, only one of the surveyed languages required more [[morphology (linguistics)|morphological]] features to mark inalienable possession than alienable possession.<ref name="auto1"/> If a language makes a grammatical distinction between alienable and inalienable nouns, having an overt possessive marker to mark inalienability is redundant. After all, by being inalienable, a noun must be possessed. ===Tighter structural bond between possessor and possessee=== In inalienable possession constructions, the relationship between the possessor and possessee is stronger than in alienable possession constructions. [[Johanna Nichols]] characterizes that by the tendency of inalienable possession to be [[head-marking language|head-marked]] but alienable possession to be [[dependent-marking language|dependent-marked]].<ref name="space and time"/> In head-marking, the [[head (linguistics)|head]] of an inalienable possession construction (the possessed noun) is marked, but in dependent-marking, the dependent (the possessor noun) is marked.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Matthews|first1=P. H.|title=Head marking|publisher=Oxford University Press|doi=10.1093/acref/9780199202720.001.0001|year=2007|isbn=9780199202720}}</ref>
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)