Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Origin of language
(section)
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
== Linguistic structures == === Lexical-phonological principle === Hockett (1966) details a list of features regarded as essential to describing human language.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Hockett |first=Charles F. |year=1960 |title=The Origin of Speech |url=http://www.gifted.ucalgary.ca/dflynn/files/dflynn/Hockett60.pdf |url-status=dead |journal=Scientific American |volume=203 |issue=3 |pages=88–96 |bibcode=1960SciAm.203c..88H |doi=10.1038/scientificamerican0960-88 |pmid=14402211 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20140106173517/http://www.gifted.ucalgary.ca/dflynn/files/dflynn/Hockett60.pdf |archive-date=6 January 2014 |access-date=6 January 2014}}</ref> In the domain of the lexical-phonological principle, two features of this list are most important: * [[Productivity (linguistics)|Productivity]]: users can create and understand completely novel messages. ** New messages are freely coined by blending, analogizing from, or transforming old ones. ** Either new or old elements are freely assigned new semantic loads by circumstances and context. This says that in every language, new idioms constantly come into existence. * Duality (of Patterning): a large number of meaningful elements are made up of a conveniently small number of independently meaningless yet message-differentiating elements. The sound system of a language is composed of a finite set of simple phonological items. Under the specific [[phonotactic]] rules of a given language, these items can be recombined and concatenated, giving rise to [[Morphology (linguistics)|morphology]] and the open-ended lexicon. A key feature of language is that a simple, finite set of phonological items gives rise to an infinite lexical system wherein rules determine the form of each item, and meaning is inextricably linked with form. Phonological syntax, then, is a simple combination of pre-existing phonological units. Related to this is another essential feature of human language: lexical syntax, wherein pre-existing units are combined, giving rise to semantically novel or distinct lexical items.{{Citation needed paragraph|date=January 2014}} Certain elements of the lexical-phonological principle are known to exist outside of humans. While all (or nearly all) have been documented in some form in the natural world, very few coexist within the same species. Bird-song, singing nonhuman apes, and the songs of whales all display phonological syntax, combining units of sound into larger structures apparently devoid of enhanced or novel meaning. Certain other primate species do have simple phonological systems with units referring to entities in the world. However, in contrast to human systems, the units in these primates' systems normally occur in isolation, betraying a lack of lexical syntax. There is new{{When|date=May 2021}} evidence to suggest that [[Campbell's monkey|Campbell's monkeys]] also display lexical syntax, combining two calls (a predator alarm call with a "boom", the combination of which denotes a lessened threat of danger), however it is still unclear whether this is a lexical or a morphological phenomenon.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Schlenker |first1=Philippe |last2=Chemla |first2=Emmanuel |last3=Arnold |first3=Kate |last4=Lemasson |first4=Alban |last5=Ouattara |first5=Karim |last6=Keenan |first6=Sumir |last7=Stephan |first7=Claudia |last8=Ryder |first8=Robin |last9=Zuberbühler |first9=Klaus |date=December 2014 |title=Monkey semantics: two 'dialects' of Campbell's monkey alarm calls |journal=Linguistics and Philosophy |volume=37 |issue=6 |pages=439–501 |doi=10.1007/s10988-014-9155-7 |s2cid=3428900}}</ref> === Pidgins and creoles === {{Main|Creole language|pidgin}} [[Pidgin]]s are significantly simplified languages with only rudimentary grammar and a restricted vocabulary. In their early stage, pidgins mainly consist of nouns, verbs, and adjectives with few or no articles, prepositions, conjunctions or auxiliary verbs. Often the grammar has no fixed [[word order]] and the words have no [[inflection]].<ref name="Diamond1992">{{Cite book |last=Diamond |first=Jared M. |title=The third chimpanzee : the evolution and future of the human animal |publisher=HarperCollins |year=1992 |isbn=978-0-06-018307-3 |location=New York |pages=[https://archive.org/details/thirdchimpanzee00jare_0/page/141 141–167] |chapter=Bridges to human language |chapter-url=https://archive.org/details/thirdchimpanzee00jare_0/page/141}}</ref> If contact is maintained between the groups speaking the pidgin for long periods of time, the pidgins may become more complex over many generations. If the children of one generation adopt the pidgin as their native language it develops into a [[creole language]], which becomes fixed and acquires a more complex grammar, with fixed phonology, syntax, morphology, and syntactic embedding. The syntax and morphology of such languages may often have local innovations not obviously derived from any of the parent languages. Studies of creole languages around the world have suggested that they display remarkable similarities in grammar{{Citation needed|date=December 2018}} and are developed uniformly from pidgins in a single generation. These similarities are apparent even when creoles do not have any common language origins. In addition, creoles are similar, despite being developed in isolation from each other. [[Language bioprogram theory|Syntactic similarities]] include [[subject–verb–object]] word order. Even when creoles are derived from languages with a different word order they often develop the SVO word order. Creoles tend to have similar usage patterns for definite and indefinite articles, and similar movement rules for phrase structures even when the parent languages do not.<ref name="Diamond1992" />
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)