Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Consonant cluster
(section)
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
== Clusters in languages == All languages differ in syllable structure and cluster template. A loanword from [[Adyghe language|Adyghe]] in the extinct [[Ubykh language]], {{lang|uby|psta}} ('to well up'), violates Ubykh's limit of two initial consonants. The English words ''sphere'' {{IPA|/ˈsfɪər/}} and ''sphinx'' {{IPA|/ˈsfɪŋks/}}, [[Greek language|Greek]] loanwords, break the rule that two fricatives may not appear adjacently [[word-initially]]. Some English words, including thrash, three, throat, and throw, start with the voiceless dental fricative /θ/, the liquid /r/, or the /r/ cluster (/θ/+/r/). This cluster example in Proto-Germanic has a counterpart in which /θ/ was followed by /l/. In early North and West Germanic, the /l/ cluster disappeared. This suggests that clusters are affected as words are loaned to other languages. The examples show that every language has syllable preference<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Dziubalska-Kołaczyk |first1=K. |title=On the structure, survival and change of consonant clusters |journal=Folia Linguistica |date=2019 |volume=53(s40-s1) |issue=53 |page=107|doi=10.1515/flih-2019-0006 |s2cid=199546117 }}</ref> based on syllable structure and segment harmony of the language. Other factors that affect clusters when loaned to other languages include speech rate, articulatory factors, and speech perceptivity.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Côté |first1=M. |title=Consonant cluster simplification in Québec French |date=2004 |journal=Probus |volume=16 |issue=2 |page=151 |doi=10.1515/prbs.2004.16.2.151|s2cid=170972749 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Guy |first1=G. R. |last2=Boyd |first2=S. |title=The development of a morphological class |journal=Language Variation and Change |date=1990 |volume=2 |issue=1 |page=1|doi=10.1017/S0954394500000235 |s2cid=145289008 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Wilson |first1=C. |title=Consonant cluster neutralisation and targeted constraints |journal=Phonology |date=2001 |volume=18 |issue=1 |page=147|doi=10.1017/S0952675701004043 |s2cid=62574684 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Jun |first1=J. |title=Positional effects in consonant clusters |journal=The Blackwell Companion to Phonology |date=2011 |volume=2 |page=1103 }}</ref> Bayley has added that social factors such as age, gender, and geographical locations of speakers can determine clusters when they are loaned crosslinguistically.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Bayley |first1=R. |title=Consonant cluster reduction in Tejano English |journal=Language Variation and Change |date=1994 |volume=6 |issue=3 |page=303|doi=10.1017/S0954394500001708 |s2cid=145407662 }}</ref>
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)