Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Cued speech
(section)
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
==Literacy== Cued speech is based on the hypothesis that if all the sounds in the spoken language looked clearly different from each other on the lips of the speaker, people with a hearing loss would learn a language in much the same way as a hearing person, but through vision rather than audition.<ref name="Cornett">Cued Speech: What and Why?, R. Orin Cornett, Ph.D., undated white paper.</ref><ref name="Sweden">Proceedings of the International Congress on Education of the Deaf, Stockholm, Sweden 1970, Vol. 1, pp. 97-99</ref> Literacy is the ability to read and write proficiently, which allows one to understand and communicate ideas so as to participate in a literate society. Cued speech was designed to help eliminate the difficulties of English language acquisition and literacy development in children who are deaf or hard-of-hearing. Results of research show that accurate and consistent cueing with a child can help in the development of language, communication and literacy but its importance and use is debated. Studies address the issues behind literacy development,<ref>{{Cite web |last=Schwartz |first=Sue |date=2007 |title=Choices in Deafness: A Parents' Guide to Communication Options by Sue Schwartz (Editor), Ph.D. (Editor) " {{!}} 9781890627737 {{!}} Get Textbooks {{!}} New Textbooks {{!}} Used Textbooks {{!}} College Textbooks - GetTextbooks.co.uk" |url=https://www.gettextbooks.co.uk/isbn/9781890627737/ |access-date=2023-03-01 |website=www.gettextbooks.co.uk}}</ref> traditional deaf education, and how using cued speech affects the lives of deaf and [[Hearing loss|HOH]] children. Cued speech does indeed achieve its goal of distinguishing phonemes received by the learner, but there is some question of whether it is as helpful to expression as it is to reception. An article by Jacqueline Leybaert and Jesús Alegría discusses how children who are introduced to cued speech before the age of one are up-to-speed with their hearing peers on receptive vocabulary, though expressive vocabulary lags behind.<ref name="leybaert">{{Cite book |last1=Leybaert |first1=Jacqueline |url=https://public.ebookcentral.proquest.com/choice/publicfullrecord.aspx?p=1886784 |title=Cued speech and cued language for deaf and hard of hearing children |last2=LaSasso |first2=Carol |last3=Crain |first3=Kelly Lamar |last4=ProQuest (Firm) |date=2010 |publisher=Plural Pub |isbn=978-1-59756-334-5 |location=San Diego, CA}}</ref> The writers suggest additional and separate training to teach oral expression if such is desired, but more importantly this reflects the nature of cued speech; to adapt children who are deaf and hard-of-hearing to a hearing world, as such discontinuities of expression and reception are not as commonly found for children with a hearing loss who are learning sign language.<ref name=leybaert/> In her paper "The Relationship Between Phonological Coding And Reading Achievement In Deaf Children: Is Cued Speech A Special Case?" (1998), Ostrander notes, "Research has consistently shown a link between lack of phonological awareness and reading disorders (Jenkins & Bowen, 1994)" and discusses the research basis for teaching cued speech as an aid to phonological awareness and literacy.<ref>http://web.syr.edu/~clostran/literacy.html {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20060503173056/http://web.syr.edu/~clostran/literacy.html |date=2006-05-03 }} "The Relationship Between Phonological Coding And Reading Achievement In Deaf Children: Is Cued Speech A Special Case?" Carolyn Ostrander, 1998 (accessed August 23, 2006)</ref> Ostrander concludes that further research into these areas is needed and well justified.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Nielsen |first1=Diane Corcoran |last2=Luetke-Stahlman |first2=Barbara |date=2002 |title=Phonological Awareness: One Key to the Reading Proficiency of Deaf Children |url=https://www.jstor.org/stable/44390352 |journal=American Annals of the Deaf |volume=147 |issue=3 |pages=11–19 |jstor=44390352 |issn=0002-726X}}</ref> The editor of the ''[[Editor|Cued Speech Journal]]'' (currently sought but not discovered) reports that "Research indicating that Cued Speech does greatly improve the reception of spoken language by profoundly deaf children was reported in 1979 by Gaye Nicholls, and in 1982 by Nicholls and Ling."<ref>Editor Carol J. Boggs, Ph.D, "Editor's Notes", ''Cued Speech Journal'', (1990) Vol 4, pg ii</ref> In the book ''Choices in Deafness: A Parents' Guide to Communication Options'', [[Sue Schwartz]] writes on how cued speech helps a deaf child recognize pronunciation. The child can learn how to pronounce words such as "hors d'oeuvre" or "tamale" or "Hermione" that have pronunciations different from how they are spelled. A child can learn about accents and dialects. In New York, coffee may be pronounced "caw fee"; in the South, the word friend ("fray-end") can be a two-syllable word.<ref>Sue Schwartz, Ph.D, Choices in Deafness: A Parents' Guide to Communication Options</ref>
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)