Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Problem solving
(section)
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
=== Cognitive sciences === Among the first experimental psychologists to study problem solving were the [[Gestalt psychology|Gestaltists]] in [[Germany]], such as [[Karl Duncker]] in ''The Psychology of Productive Thinking'' (1935).<ref name=Duncker>{{cite book | last= Duncker|first=Karl |year=1935 |title=Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens |trans-title=The psychology of productive thinking |location=Berlin |publisher=Julius Springer |language=de}}</ref> Perhaps best known is the work of [[Allen Newell]] and [[Herbert A. Simon]].<ref name="Newell">{{cite book |last1=Newell|first1= Allen |last2=Simon|first2= Herbert A. |year=1972 |title=Human problem solving |location=Englewood Cliffs, N.J. |publisher=Prentice-Hall}}</ref> Experiments in the 1960s and early 1970s asked participants to solve relatively simple, well-defined, but not previously seen laboratory tasks.<ref>For example: * X-ray problem, by {{cite book | last= Duncker|first= Karl |year=1935 |title=Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens |trans-title=The psychology of productive thinking |place=Berlin |publisher=Julius Springer |language=de}} * Disk problem, later known as [[Tower of Hanoi]], by {{cite journal | last1=Ewert | first1=P. H. | last2=Lambert | first2=J. F. | title=Part II: The Effect of Verbal Instructions upon the Formation of a Concept | journal=The Journal of General Psychology | publisher=Informa UK Limited | volume=6 | issue=2 | year=1932 | issn=0022-1309 | doi=10.1080/00221309.1932.9711880 | pages=400β413 | url=https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/00221309.1932.9711880 | url-access=subscription | access-date=2019-06-09 | archive-date=2020-08-06 | archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20200806135752/https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/00221309.1932.9711880 | url-status=live }}</ref><ref>{{cite book | last= Mayer|first= R. E. |year=1992 |title=Thinking, problem solving, cognition |edition=Second |location=New York |publisher=W. H. Freeman and Company}}</ref> These simple problems, such as the [[Tower of Hanoi]], admitted [[optimal solution]]s that could be found quickly, allowing researchers to observe the full problem-solving process. Researchers assumed that these model problems would elicit the characteristic [[cognitive process]]es by which more complex "real world" problems are solved. An outstanding problem-solving technique found by this research is the principle of [[Decomposition (computer science)|decomposition]].<ref>{{cite journal |first1=J. Scott|last1=Armstrong|first2=William B. Jr.|last2=Denniston|first3=Matt M.|last3=Gordon |year=1975|title=The Use of the Decomposition Principle in Making Judgments |url=http://marketing.wharton.upenn.edu/ideas/pdf/armstrong2/DecompositionPrinciple.pdf |journal=Organizational Behavior and Human Performance |volume=14 |issue=2 |pages=257β263 |doi=10.1016/0030-5073(75)90028-8 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20100620221713/http://marketing.wharton.upenn.edu/ideas/pdf/armstrong2/DecompositionPrinciple.pdf |archive-date=2010-06-20 |s2cid=122659209}}</ref>
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)