Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Perception
(section)
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
== Effects on perception == === Effect of experience === {{main|Perceptual learning}}With experience, [[organism]]s can learn to make finer perceptual distinctions, and learn new kinds of categorization. Wine-tasting, the reading of X-ray images and music appreciation are applications of this process in the [[human]] sphere. [[Research]] has focused on the relation of this to other kinds of [[learning]], and whether it takes place in peripheral [[Sensory perception|sensory]] systems or in the brain's processing of sense information.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Sumner |first1=Meghan |last2=Samuel |first2=Arthur G. |url=https://web.stanford.edu/~sumner/Publications/2009_Sumner_JML.pdf |title=The Effect of Experience on the Perception and Representation of Dialect Variants |journal=Journal of Memory and Language |publisher=[[Elsevier Inc.]] |date=May 2009 |volume=60 |issue=4 |pages=487β501 |doi=10.1016/j.jml.2009.01.001 |access-date=3 June 2015 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20160202011511/https://web.stanford.edu/~sumner/Publications/2009_Sumner_JML.pdf |archive-date=2 February 2016 |url-status=live}}</ref> [[Empirical]] [[research]] show that specific practices (such as [[yoga]], [[mindfulness]], [[Tai chi|Tai Chi]], [[meditation]], Daoshi and other mind-body disciplines) can modify human perceptual modality. Specifically, these practices enable perception skills to switch from the external (exteroceptive field) towards a higher ability to focus on internal signals (''[[proprioception]]''). Also, when asked to provide verticality judgments, highly self-transcendent [[yoga]] practitioners were significantly less influenced by a misleading visual context. Increasing self-transcendence may enable yoga practitioners to optimize verticality judgment tasks by relying more on internal (vestibular and proprioceptive) signals coming from their own body, rather than on exteroceptive, visual cues.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Fiori |first1=Francesca |last2=David |first2=Nicole |last3=Aglioti |first3=Salvatore Maria |date=2014 |title=Processing of proprioceptive and vestibular body signals and self-transcendence in Ashtanga yoga practitioners |journal=[[Frontiers in Human Neuroscience]] |volume=8 |pages=734 |doi=10.3389/fnhum.2014.00734 |pmc=4166896 |pmid=25278866 |doi-access=free}}</ref> Past actions and events that transpire right before an encounter or any form of stimulation have a strong degree of influence on how sensory stimuli are processed and perceived. On a basic level, the information our senses receive is often ambiguous and incomplete. However, they are grouped together in order for us to be able to understand the physical world around us. But it is these various forms of stimulation, combined with our previous knowledge and experience that allows us to create our overall perception. For example, when engaging in conversation, we attempt to understand their message and words by not only paying attention to what we hear through our ears but also from the previous shapes we have seen our mouths make. Another example would be if we had a similar topic come up in another conversation, we would use our previous knowledge to guess the direction the conversation is headed in.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Snyder |first=Joel |date=31 October 2015 |pmc=4628108 |title=How previous experience shapes perception in different sensory modalities |journal=Frontiers in Human Neuroscience |volume=9 |pages=594 |pmid=26582982 |doi=10.3389/fnhum.2015.00594 |doi-access=free }}</ref> === Effect of motivation and expectation === {{main|Set (psychology)}}A ''perceptual set'' (also called ''perceptual expectancy'' or simply ''set'') is a predisposition to perceive things in a certain way.<ref name="Weiten2008">{{cite book|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=sILajOhJpOsC&pg=PT193|title=Psychology: Themes and Variations|last=Weiten|first=Wayne|date=17 December 2008|publisher=Cengage Learning|isbn=978-0-495-60197-5|page=193|access-date=24 March 2011|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20111109163032/http://books.google.com/books?id=sILajOhJpOsC&pg=PT193|archive-date=9 November 2011|url-status=live}}</ref> It is an example of how perception can be shaped by "top-down" processes such as drives and expectations.<ref name="CoonMitterer2008">{{cite book|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=vw20LEaJe10C&pg=PA171|title=Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and Behavior|last1=Coon|first1=Dennis|last2=Mitterer|first2=John O.|date=29 December 2008|publisher=Cengage Learning|isbn=978-0-495-59911-1|pages=171β172|access-date=24 March 2011|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20111109162432/http://books.google.com/books?id=vw20LEaJe10C&pg=PA171|archive-date=9 November 2011|url-status=live}}</ref> Perceptual sets occur in all the different senses.<ref name="Sonderegger1998" /> They can be long term, such as a special sensitivity to hearing one's own name in a crowded room, or short-term, as in the ease with which hungry people notice the smell of food.<ref name="HardyHeyes1999">{{cite book|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=fjPWqXi9WQsC&pg=PA24|title=Beginning Psychology|last1=Hardy|first1=Malcolm|last2=Heyes|first2=Steve|date=2 December 1999|publisher=Oxford University Press|isbn=978-0-19-832821-6|pages=24β27|access-date=24 March 2011|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20111109163023/http://books.google.com/books?id=fjPWqXi9WQsC&pg=PA24|archive-date=9 November 2011|url-status=live}}</ref> A simple demonstration of the effect involved very brief presentations of non-words such as "sael". Subjects who were told to expect words about animals read it as "seal", but others who were expecting boat-related words read it as "sail".<ref name="HardyHeyes1999" /> Sets can be created by [[motivation]] and so can result in people interpreting ambiguous figures so that they see what they want to see.<ref name="CoonMitterer2008" /> For instance, how someone perceives what unfolds during a sports game can be biased if they strongly support one of the teams.<ref name="BlockYuker2002">{{cite book|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=uNMFiMQu8BMC&pg=PA173|title=Can You Believe Your Eyes?: Over 250 Illusions and Other Visual Oddities|last1=Block|first1=J. R.|last2=Yuker|first2=Harold E.|date=1 October 2002|publisher=Robson|isbn=978-1-86105-586-6|pages=173β174|access-date=24 March 2011|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20111109162334/http://books.google.com/books?id=uNMFiMQu8BMC&pg=PA173|archive-date=9 November 2011|url-status=live}}</ref> In one experiment, students were allocated to pleasant or unpleasant tasks by a computer. They were told that either a number or a letter would flash on the screen to say whether they were going to taste an orange juice drink or an unpleasant-tasting health drink. In fact, an ambiguous figure was flashed on screen, which could either be read as the letter B or the number 13. When the letters were associated with the pleasant task, subjects were more likely to perceive a letter B, and when letters were associated with the unpleasant task they tended to perceive a number 13.<ref name="Weiten2008" /> Perceptual set has been demonstrated in many social contexts. When someone has a reputation for being funny, an audience is more likely to find them amusing.<ref name="HardyHeyes1999" /> Individual's perceptual sets reflect their own personality traits. For example, people with an aggressive personality are quicker to correctly identify aggressive words or situations.<ref name="HardyHeyes1999" /> In general, perceptual speed as a mental ability is positively correlated with personality traits such as conscientiousness, emotional stability, and agreeableness suggesting its evolutionary role in preserving homeostasis.<ref>{{Cite book |last=Stanek |first=Kevin |url=https://umnlibraries.manifoldapp.org/projects/of-anchors-and-sails |title=Of Anchors & Sails: Personality-Ability Trait Constellations |last2=Ones |first2=Deniz |date=2023-11-20 |publisher=University of Minnesota |isbn=978-1-946135-98-8 |doi=10.24926/9781946135988}}</ref> One classic psychological experiment showed slower reaction times and less accurate answers when a deck of [[playing cards]] reversed the color of the [[Suit (cards)|suit]] symbol for some cards (e.g. red spades and black hearts).<ref>"On the Perception of Incongruity: A Paradigm" by Jerome S. Bruner and Leo Postman. ''Journal of Personality'', 18, pp. 206-223. 1949. [http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Bruner/Cards/ Yorku.ca] {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20060215174339/http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Bruner/Cards/|date=15 February 2006}}</ref> Philosopher [[Andy Clark]] explains that perception, although it occurs quickly, is not simply a bottom-up process (where minute details are put together to form larger wholes). Instead, our brains use what he calls ''[[predictive coding]]''. It starts with very broad constraints and expectations for the state of the world, and as expectations are met, it makes more detailed predictions (errors lead to new predictions, or ''[[learning]] processes)''. Clark says this research has various implications; not only can there be no completely "unbiased, unfiltered" perception, but this means that there is a great deal of feedback between perception and expectation (perceptual experiences often shape our beliefs, but those perceptions were based on existing beliefs).<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.edge.org/q2011/q11_6.html|title=Predictive Coding|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20131205173145/http://www.edge.org/q2011/q11_6.html|archive-date=5 December 2013|url-status=dead|access-date=24 February 2011}}</ref> Indeed, predictive coding provides an account where this type of feedback assists in stabilizing our inference-making process about the physical world, such as with perceptual constancy examples. [[Embodied cognition]] challenges the idea of perception as internal representations resulting from a passive reception of (incomplete) sensory inputs coming from the outside world. According to O'Regan (1992), the major issue with this perspective is that it leaves the subjective character of perception unexplained.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=O'Regan |first=J. Kevin |date=1992 |title=Solving the "real" mysteries of visual perception: The world as an outside memory. |url=http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/h0084327 |journal=Canadian Journal of Psychology |language=en |volume=46 |issue=3 |pages=461β488 |doi=10.1037/h0084327 |pmid=1486554 |issn=0008-4255|url-access=subscription }}</ref> Thus, perception is understood as an active process conducted by perceiving and engaged agents (perceivers). Furthermore, perception is influenced by agents' motives and expectations, their bodily states, and the interaction between the agent's body and the environment around it.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=O'Regan |first1=J. Kevin |last2=NoΓ« |first2=Alva |date=2001 |title=A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness |url=https://www.cambridge.org/core/product/identifier/S0140525X01000115/type/journal_article |journal=Behavioral and Brain Sciences |language=en |volume=24 |issue=5 |pages=939β973 |doi=10.1017/S0140525X01000115 |pmid=12239892 |issn=0140-525X|url-access=subscription }}</ref>
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)