Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Conversation analysis
(section)
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
===Turn-taking organization=== {{Main|Turn-taking}} The analysis of turn-taking started with the description in a model in the paper known as the ''Simplest Systematics'',<ref name="Sacks1974">{{cite journal |last1=Sacks |first1=Harvey |last2=Schegloff |first2=Emanuel A. |last3=Jefferson |first3=Gail |title=A Simplest Systematics for the Organization of Turn-Taking for Conversation |journal=Language |date=1974 |volume=50 |issue=4 |pages=696β735 |doi=10.2307/412243|jstor=412243 |hdl=11858/00-001M-0000-002C-4337-3 |url=http://pubman.mpdl.mpg.de/pubman/item/escidoc:2376846/component/escidoc:2376845/Sacks_1974_simplest_systematics.pdf|access-date=24 December 2021}}</ref> which was very programmatic for the field of Conversation analysis and one of the most cited papers published in the journal [[Language (journal)|''Language'']].<ref>{{cite web |title=The Most Cited Language Articles (1925-2012) in the First Half of 2017 |url=https://www.linguisticsociety.org/news/2017/07/11/most-cited-language-articles-1925-2012-first-half-2017 |website=www.linguisticsociety.org |access-date=25 November 2021 |date=11 July 2017}}</ref> The model is designed to explain that when people talk in conversation, they do not always talk all at the same time, but generally, one person speaks at a time, and then another person can follow.<ref name="Sacks1974"/> Such a contribution to a conversation by one speaker is then a ''turn''. A turn is created through certain forms or units that listeners can recognize and count on, called ''[[turn construction unit]]s'' (TCUs), and speakers and listeners will know that such forms can be a word or a clause, and use that knowledge to predict when a speaker is finished so that others can speak, to avoid or minimize both overlap and silence. A listener will look for the places where they can start speaking - so-called ''transition relevant places'' (TRPs) - based on how the units appear over time. Turn construction units can be created or recognized via four methods, i.e. types of unit design:<ref>{{cite book |last1=Ford |first1=Cecilia E. |last2=Thompson |first2=Sandra A. |chapter=Interactional units in conversation: syntactic, intonational, and pragmatic resources for the management of turns |title=Interaction and Grammar |date=1996 |pages=134β184 |doi=10.1017/CBO9780511620874.003|isbn=9780521552257 }}</ref> * [[Grammar|Grammatical]] methods, i.e. morphosyntactic structures. * [[Prosody (linguistics)|Prosodic]] methods, e.g. pitch, speed and changes in pronunciation. * Pragmatic methods: turns perform actions, and at the point where listeners have heard enough and know enough, a turn can be pragmatically complete. * Visual methods: Gesture, gaze and body movement is also used to indicate that a turn is over. For example, a person speaking looks at the next speaker when their turn is about to end. Each time a turn is over, speakers also have to decide who can talk next, and this is called turn allocation. The rules for turn allocation is commonly formulated in the same way as in the original Simplest Systematics paper, with 2 parts where the first consists of 3 elements: #* a. If the current speaker selects a next one to speak at the end of current TCU (by name, gaze or contextual aspects of what is said), the selected speaker has the right and obligation to speak next. #* b. If the current speaker does not select a next speaker, other potential speakers have the right to self-select (the first starter gets the turn) #* c. If options 1a and 1b have not been implemented, current speaker may continue with another TCU. # At the end of that TCU, the option system applies again. Based on the turn-taking system, three types of silence may be distinguished: * Pause: A period of silence within a speaker's TCU, i.e. during a speaker's turn when a sentence is not finished. * Gap: A period of silence between turns, for example after a question has been asked and not yet answered * Lapse: A period of silence when no sequence or other structured activity is in progress: the current speaker stops talking, does not select a next speaker, and no one self selects. Lapses are commonly associated with visual or other forms of disengagement between speakers, even if these periods are brief. Some types of turns may require extra work before they can successfully take place. Speakers wanting a long turn, for example to tell a story or describe important news, must first establish that others will not intervene during the course of the telling through some form of preface and approval by the listener (a so-called ''go-ahead''). The preface and its associated go-ahead comprise a ''pre-sequence''.<ref name="Schegloff2007">{{cite book |last=Schegloff |first=Emanuel A. |title=Sequence organization in interaction: a primer in conversation analysis |date=2007 |publisher=Cambridge University Press |location=Cambridge, UK |isbn=9780511791208|doi=10.1017/CBO9780511791208}}</ref><ref>{{cite book|last=Harvey |first=Sacks|editor1-last=Sherzer |editor1-first=Joel |editor2-last=Bauman |editor2-first=Richard |title=Explorations in the Ethnography of Speaking|date=1974|publisher=Cambridge University Press |location=Cambridge|pages=337β353|chapter=An analysis of the course of a joke's telling in conversation}}</ref> Conversations cannot be appropriately ended by 'just stopping', but require a special closing sequence.<ref name="Schegloff1973" /> The model also leaves puzzles to be solved, for example concerning how turn boundaries are identified and projected, and the role played by gaze and body orientation in the management of turn-taking. It also establishes some questions for other disciplines: for example, the split second timing of turn-transition sets up a cognitive 'bottle neck' in which potential speakers must attend to incoming speech while also preparing their own contribution - something which imposes a heavy load of human processing capacity, and which may impact the structure of languages.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Christiansen |first1=Morten H. |last2=Chater |first2=Nick |title=The Now-or-Never bottleneck: A fundamental constraint on language |journal=Behavioral and Brain Sciences |date=2016 |volume=39 |pages=e62 |doi=10.1017/S0140525X1500031X|pmid=25869618 |s2cid=54524760 }}</ref> However, the original formulation in Sacks et al.1974 is designed to model turn-taking only in ordinary and informal conversation, and not interaction in more specialized, institutional environments such as meetings, courts, news interviews, mediation hearings, which have distinctive turn-taking organizations that depart in various ways from ordinary conversation. Later studies have looked at institutional interaction and turn-taking in institutional contexts. Interruptions have also been examined and analyzed.<ref>{{Cite book |last=Beattie |first=Geoffrey |title=Talk: An Analysis of Speech and Non-Verbal Behaviour in Conversation |publisher=Open University Press |year=1983 |isbn=0-335-10414-2 |location=Milton Keynes |pages=110ff |language=en}}</ref>
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)