Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Prevalence
(section)
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
==Limitations== It can be said that a very small error applied over a very large number of individuals (that is, those who are ''not affected'' by the condition in the general population during their lifetime; for example, over 95%) produces a relevant, non-negligible number of subjects who are incorrectly classified as having the condition or any other condition which is the object of a survey study: these subjects are the so-called false positives; such reasoning applies to the 'false positive' but not the 'false negative' problem where we have an error applied over a relatively very small number of individuals to begin with (that is, those who are ''affected'' by the condition in the general population; for example, less than 5%). Hence, a very high percentage of subjects who seem to have a history of a disorder at interview are false positives for such a medical condition and apparently never developed a fully clinical [[syndrome]].{{citation needed|date=July 2021}} A different but related problem in evaluating the public health significance of psychiatric conditions has been highlighted by [[Robert Spitzer (psychiatrist)|Robert Spitzer]] of [[Columbia University]]: fulfillment of [[diagnostic criteria]] and the resulting [[medical diagnosis|diagnosis]] do not necessarily imply need for treatment.<ref name=Spitzer1998>{{cite journal | first = Robert | last = Spitzer | date = February 1998 | title = Diagnosis and need for treatment are not the same | journal = Archives of General Psychiatry | volume = 55 | issue = 2 | pages = 120 | url = http://archpsyc.ama-assn.org/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=9477924 | doi = 10.1001/archpsyc.55.2.120 | pmid = 9477924 | url-status = dead | archive-url = http://archive.wikiwix.com/cache/20110705210403/http://archpsyc.ama-assn.org/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=9477924 | archive-date = 2011-07-05 | url-access = subscription }}</ref> A well-known statistical problem arises when ascertaining rates for disorders and conditions with a relatively low population prevalence or [[base rate]]. Even assuming that lay interview diagnoses are highly accurate in terms of [[Sensitivity (tests)|sensitivity]] and [[Specificity (tests)|specificity]] and their corresponding area under the [[ROC curve]] (that is, [[Area under the curve|AUC]], or area under the [[receiver operating characteristic]] curve), a condition with a relatively low prevalence or base-rate is bound to yield high [[Type I and type II errors|false positive]] rates, which exceed [[Type I and type II errors|false negative]] rates; in such a circumstance a limited [[positive predictive value]], PPV, yields high [[false positive]] rates even in presence of a specificity which is very close to 100%.<ref name=Baldessarini1993>{{cite journal | first = Ross J. | last = Baldessarini |author2=Finklestein S. |author3=Arana G. W. |date=May 1983 | title =The predictive power of diagnostic tests and the effect of prevalence of illness | journal = Archives of General Psychiatry | volume = 40 | issue = 5 | pages = 569β73 |pmid=6838334 | doi=10.1001/archpsyc.1983.01790050095011}}</ref>
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)