Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Interference theory
(section)
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
===Iconic research=== ====Modified (free) recall==== Briggs's (1954) study modeled McGeoch's work on interference by setting the stage for a classic design of retroactive interference. In his study, participants were asked to learn 12 paired associates to a criterion of 100%. To ensure parsimony, these pairs can be labeled as A<sub>1</sub>-B<sub>1</sub>-, A<sub>2</sub>-B<sub>2</sub>-...A<sub> I </sub>-B<sub> I </sub> (also called AB/AC paradigm). Briggs used a "modified free recall" technique by asking participants to recall an item when cued with B<sub> I </sub>. Over multiple anticipation trials, participants learned B<sub> I </sub> items through the prompt of B<sub> I </sub> items. After perfecting A<sub> I </sub>- B<sub> I </sub> learning, participants were given a new list of paired associates to learn; however B<sub> I </sub> items were replaced with C<sub> I </sub> items (now given a list of A<sub>1</sub>-C<sub>1</sub>-, A<sub>2</sub>-C<sub>2</sub>-...A<sub> I </sub>-C<sub> I </sub>). As the learning of A<sub> I </sub>-C<sub> I </sub> pairs increased, the learning of A<sub> I </sub>-B<sub> I </sub> pairs decreased. Eventually recalling the C<sub> I </sub> items exceeded the recall of the B<sub> I </sub> items, representing the phenomenon of retroactive interference. A significant part of Briggs's (1954) study was that once participants were tested after a delay of 24 hours the Bi responses spontaneously recovered and exceeded the recall of the Ci items. Briggs explained the [[spontaneous recovery]] illustration as an account of A<sub> I </sub>-B<sub> I </sub> items competing with A<sub> I </sub>-C<sub> I </sub> items or, as McGeoch would define it: "a resultant [of] momentary dominance".<ref name = Briggs>{{cite journal | last1 = Briggs | first1 = G. E. | year = 1954 | title = Acquisition, extinction, and recovery functions in retroactive inhibition | journal = Journal of Experimental Psychology | volume = 47 | issue = 5| pages = 285β293 | doi=10.1037/h0060251 | pmid = 13163344}}</ref> ====Modified modified free recall==== J.M. Barnes and B.J. Underwood (1959) expanded Briggs's (1954) study by implementing a similar procedure. The main difference in this study, however, was that, unlike Briggs's (1954) "modified free recall" (MFR) task where participants gave one-item responses, Barnes and Underwood asked participants to give both List 1 and List 2 responses to each cued recall task. Participants' ability to recall both items was termed the "modified modified free recall" (MMFR) technique. Equivocally to Briggs's (1954) results, RI occurred when C<sub> I </sub> recalled responses gradually came to exceed B<sub> I </sub> responses. Barnes and Underwood argued that because there was "unlimited recall time" to produce multiple-item responses, the fact that A<sub> I </sub>-C<sub> I </sub> responses still trumped A<sub> I </sub>-B<sub> I </sub> responses represented an account of unlearning.<ref name = barns>{{cite journal | last1 = Barnes | first1 = J. M | last2 = Underwood | first2 = B. J. | year = 1959 | title = Fate of first-list associations in transfer theory | journal = Journal of Experimental Psychology | volume = 58 | issue = 2| pages = 97β105 | doi=10.1037/h0047507| pmid = 13796886 }}</ref>
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)