Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Afterimage
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
{{short description|Image that continues to appear in the eyes after a period of exposure to the original image}} {{Other uses}} {{More citations needed|date=January 2012}} [[File:Florence Devouard, afterimage.jpg|thumb|If a viewer stares at the white dot in the center of this image for 5–60 seconds and then looks at a plain white surface, a negative afterimage will appear, showing a person's face in a more natural color scheme. This can also be achieved by the viewer closing their eyes.]] An '''afterimage''', or '''after-image''', is an image that continues to appear in the eyes after a period of exposure to the original image. An afterimage may be a normal phenomenon (physiological afterimage) or may be pathological ([[palinopsia]]). [[Illusory palinopsia]] may be a pathological exaggeration of physiological afterimages. Afterimages occur because [[Photochemistry|photochemical]] activity in the retina continues even when the eyes are no longer experiencing the original stimulus.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Bender|first1=MB|last2=Feldman|first2=M|last3=Sobin|first3=AJ|title=Palinopsia.|journal=Brain: A Journal of Neurology|date=Jun 1968|volume=91|issue=2|pages=321–38|pmid=5721933|doi=10.1093/brain/91.2.321}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last1=Gersztenkorn|first1=D|last2=Lee|first2=AG|title=Palinopsia revamped: A systematic review of the literature.|journal=Survey of Ophthalmology|date=Jul 2, 2014|pmid=25113609|doi=10.1016/j.survophthal.2014.06.003|volume=60|issue=1|pages=1–35}}</ref> The remainder of this article refers to '''physiological afterimages'''. A common physiological afterimage is the dim area that seems to float before one's [[human eye|eye]]s after briefly looking into a light source, such as a camera flash. Palinopsia is a common symptom of [[visual snow]]. ==Negative afterimages== [[Negative image|Negative]] afterimages are generated in the retina but may be modified like other retinal signals by [[neural adaptation]] of the [[retinal ganglion cell]]s that carry signals from the [[retina]] of the eye to the rest of the brain.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Zaidi|first1= Q.|last2= Ennis|first2= R.|last3= Cao|first3= D.|last4= Lee|first4= B. |title=Neural locus of color afterimages. . |journal=Current Biology |date=2012 |volume=22 |issue=3 |pages=220–224 |doi=10.1016/j.cub.2011.12.021 |pmid=22264612 |pmc=3562597 |bibcode=2012CBio...22..220Z |url=https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.12.021 |access-date=17 October 2022|hdl=11858/00-001M-0000-000F-4AA5-4 |hdl-access=free }}</ref> Normally, any image is moved over the retina by small eye movements known as [[microsaccade]]s before much adaptation can occur. However, if the image is very intense and brief, or if the image is large, or if the eye remains very steady, these small movements cannot keep the image on unadapted parts of the retina. Afterimages can be seen when moving from a bright environment to a dim one, like walking indoors on a bright snowy day. They are accompanied by neural adaptation in the occipital lobe of the brain that function similar to [[color balance]] adjustments in photography. These adaptations attempt to keep vision consistent in dynamic lighting. Viewing a uniform background while adaptation is still occurring will allow an individual to see the afterimage because localized areas of vision are still being processed by the brain using adaptations that are no longer needed. The [[Young–Helmholtz theory|Young-Helmholtz trichromatic theory]] of color vision postulated that there were three types of photoreceptors in the eye, each sensitive to a particular range of visible light: short-wavelength cones, medium-wavelength cones, and long-wavelength cones. Trichromatic theory, however, cannot explain all afterimage phenomena. Specifically, afterimages are the complementary hue of the adapting stimulus, and trichromatic theory fails to account for this fact.<ref name=Horner>{{Cite book | author-last = Horner| author-first = David. T. | date = 2013 | chapter = Demonstrations of Color Perception and the Importance of Colors | editor1-last = Ware| editor1-first = Mark E. | editor2-last = Johnson| editor2-first = David E. | title = Handbook of Demonstrations and Activities in the Teaching of Psychology | publisher = Psychology Press | isbn = 978-1-134-99757-2 | volume = II: Physiological-Comparative, Perception, Learning, Cognitive, and Developmental | pages = 94–96 | url = https://books.google.com/books?id=iVUnAAAAQBAJ | access-date = 2019-12-06 }} Originally published as: {{Cite journal | last = Horner| first = David T. | date = 1997 | title = Demonstrations of Color Perception and the Importance of Contours | journal = Teaching of Psychology | volume = 24 | issue = 4 | pages = 267–268 | issn = 0098-6283 | doi = 10.1207/s15328023top2404_10 | s2cid = 145364769 }}</ref> The failure of trichromatic theory to account for afterimages indicates the need for an [[opponent process|opponent-process theory]] such as that articulated by [[Ewald Hering]] (1878) and further developed by Hurvich and Jameson (1957).<ref name=Horner/> The [[opponent process]] theory states that the human [[visual system]] interprets color information by processing signals from cones and rods in an antagonistic manner. The opponent color theory is that there are four opponent channels: red versus cyan, green vs magenta, blue versus yellow, and black versus white. Responses to one color of an opponent channel are antagonistic to those of the other color. Therefore, a [[green]] image will produce a [[magenta]] afterimage. The green color adapts the green channel, so they produce a weaker signal. Anything resulting in less green is interpreted as its paired primary color, which is magenta (an equal mixture of red and blue).<ref name=Horner/> ==Positive afterimages== Positive afterimages, by contrast, appear the same color as the original image. They are often very brief, lasting less than half a second. The cause of positive afterimages is not well known, but possibly reflects persisting activity in the [[visual cortex|brain]] when the [[retina]]l [[photoreceptor cell]]s continue to send neural impulses to the [[occipital lobe]].<ref>{{Cite web|url=http://www.exo.net/~pauld/activities/perception/positiveafterimage.html|title=positiveafterimage|website=www.exo.net}}</ref> A stimulus which elicits a positive image will usually trigger a negative afterimage quickly via the adaptation process. To experience this phenomenon, one can look at a bright source of light and then look away to a dark area, such as by closing the eyes. At first one should see a fading positive afterimage, likely followed by a negative afterimage that may last for much longer. It is also possible to see afterimages of random objects that are not bright, only these last for a split second and go unnoticed by most people. {{citation needed|date=March 2015}} ==On empty shape== <!-- "Color dove illusion" redirects here, so do NOT remove the illustration --> An afterimage in general is an optical illusion that refers to an image continuing to appear after exposure to the original image has ceased. Prolonged viewing of the colored patch induces an afterimage of the [[complementary color]] (for example, yellow color induces a bluish afterimage). The "afterimage on empty shape" effect is related to a class of effects referred to as [[contrast effect]]s.{{Citation needed|date=January 2012}} In this effect, an empty (white) shape is presented on a colored background for several seconds. When the background color disappears (becomes white), an illusionary color similar to the original background is [[perceived]] within the shape.{{Citation needed|date=January 2012}} The mechanism of the effect is still unclear, and may be produced by one or two of the following mechanisms: *During the presentation of the empty shape on a colored background, the colored background induces an illusory complementary color ("induced color") inside the empty shape. After the disappearance of the colored background an afterimage of the "induced color" might appear inside the "empty shape". Thus, the expected color of the shape will be complementary to the "induced color", and therefore similar to the color of the original background. *After the disappearance of the colored background, an afterimage of the background is induced. This induced color has a complementary color to that of the original background. It is possible that this background afterimage induces simultaneous contrast on the "empty shape". Simultaneous contrast is a psychophysical phenomenon of the change in the appearance of a color (or an achromatic stimulus) caused by the presence of a surrounding average color (or luminance). ==Gallery== <gallery> File:US flag(inverted).svg|The [[Flag of the United States|U.S. flag]] inverted: if a viewer stares at the middle stripe for around 25 to 30 seconds, then looks at a wall and blink rapidly, this image will appear in color. File:White_square.gif|A white square File:ItalianFlagInverted.svg|The [[Flag of Italy|Italian flag]] inverted: if a viewer stares at the middle of the flag long enough and blinks at a wall rapidly afterwards, this flag will appear in color. File:Illusion movie.ogg|Example video which produces a distorted illusion after one watches it and looks away. See [[motion aftereffect]]. </gallery> ==See also== *''[[Book of Optics]]'' *[[Emmert's law]] *[[Lilac chaser]] *[[McCollough effect]] *[[Motion aftereffect]] *[[Neural adaptation]] *[[Palinopsia]] *[[Persistence of vision]] *[[Screen burn-in]] *''[[Spectropia]]'' *[[Visual perception]] ==Notes== {{reflist}} ==External links== {{Commons category}} *[https://web.archive.org/web/20180513121241/http://www.palinopsiafoundation.org/ The Palinopsia Foundation is dedicated to increasing awareness of palinopsia, to funding research into the causes, prevention and treatments for palinopsia, and to advocating for the needs of individuals with palinopsia and their families.] *[http://www.eyeonvision.org Eye On Vision Foundation raises money and awareness for persistent visual conditions] *[http://faculty.washington.edu/chudler/after.html Afterimages], a small demonstration. *[http://www.goillusions.com/2014/08/count-black-dots-in-pic-color-illusion.html afterimage examples] {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20150618235627/http://www.goillusions.com/2014/08/count-black-dots-in-pic-color-illusion.html |date=2015-06-18 }} {{Optical illusions}} {{Visual phenomena}} [[Category:Optical illusions]] [[Category:Vision]] [[Category:Articles containing video clips]]
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Pages transcluded onto the current version of this page
(
help
)
:
Template:Citation needed
(
edit
)
Template:Cite book
(
edit
)
Template:Cite journal
(
edit
)
Template:Cite web
(
edit
)
Template:Commons category
(
edit
)
Template:More citations needed
(
edit
)
Template:Optical illusions
(
edit
)
Template:Other uses
(
edit
)
Template:Reflist
(
edit
)
Template:Short description
(
edit
)
Template:Visual phenomena
(
edit
)
Template:Webarchive
(
edit
)