Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Anterior cingulate cortex
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
{{Short description|Brain region}} {{Use dmy dates|date=January 2020}} {{Infobox brain | Name = Anterior cingulate cortex | Latin = cortex cingularis anterior | Image = Gray727 anterior cingulate cortex.png | Caption = Medial surface of left cerebral hemisphere, with anterior cingulate highlighted | Image2 = Gray727-Brodman.png | Caption2 = Medial surface of right hemisphere, with Brodmann's areas numbered | IsPartOf = | Components = | Artery = | Vein = }} In the [[human brain]], the '''anterior cingulate cortex''' ('''ACC''') is the frontal part of the [[cingulate cortex]] that resembles a "collar" surrounding the frontal part of the [[corpus callosum]]. It consists of [[Brodmann area]]s [[Brodmann area 24|24]], [[Brodmann area 32|32]], and [[Brodmann area 33|33]]. It is involved in certain higher-level functions, such as [[attention]] allocation,<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Pardo JV, Pardo PJ, Janer KW, Raichle ME | title = The anterior cingulate cortex mediates processing selection in the Stroop attentional conflict paradigm | journal = Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | volume = 87 | issue = 1 | pages = 256β9 | date = January 1990 | pmid = 2296583 | doi = 10.1073/pnas.87.1.256 | pmc = 53241 | bibcode = 1990PNAS...87..256P | doi-access = free }}</ref> [[pavlovian conditioning|reward anticipation]], [[decision-making]], impulse control (e.g. performance monitoring and error detection),<ref>{{cite web | url = http://medicalxpress.com/news/2013-03-brain-scans.html | title = Predicting repeat offenders with brain scans: You be the judge | first = John | last = Hewitt | name-list-style = vanc | publisher = medicalxpress.com | date = 26 March 2013 | access-date = 2013-03-26 }}</ref> and [[emotion]].<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Decety J, Jackson PL | title = The functional architecture of human empathy | journal = Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews | volume = 3 | issue = 2 | pages = 71β100 | date = June 2004 | pmid = 15537986 | doi = 10.1177/1534582304267187 | s2cid = 145310279 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Jackson PL, Brunet E, Meltzoff AN, Decety J | title = Empathy examined through the neural mechanisms involved in imagining how I feel versus how you feel pain | journal = Neuropsychologia | volume = 44 | issue = 5 | pages = 752β61 | year = 2006 | pmid = 16140345 | doi = 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.07.015 | citeseerx = 10.1.1.333.2783 | s2cid = 6848345 | url = http://ilabs.washington.edu/meltzoff/pdf/JacksonMeltzoff_NP06.pdf | access-date = 12 September 2019 | archive-date = 24 February 2021 | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20210224190244/http://ilabs.washington.edu/meltzoff/pdf/JacksonMeltzoff_NP06.pdf | url-status = dead }}</ref> <!-- supports [[AMCC]] dab -->Some research calls it the '''anterior midcingulate cortex''' (aMCC). [[File:MRI anterior cingulate.png|thumb|alt=Sagittal MRI slice with highlighting indicating location of the anterior cingulate cortex.|[[Saggital|Sagittal]] MRI slice with highlighting indicating location of the anterior cingulate cortex]] ==Anatomy== [[File:Anterior cingulate gyrus animation.gif|thumb|Anterior cingulate gyrus of left cerebral hemisphere, shown in red]] The anterior cingulate cortex can be divided anatomically based on cognitive ([[Dorsum (biology)|dorsal]]), and emotional ([[ventral]]) components.<ref name="Bush00">{{cite journal | vauthors = Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI | title = Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex | journal = Trends in Cognitive Sciences | volume = 4 | issue = 6 | pages = 215β222 | date = June 2000 | pmid = 10827444 | doi = 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01483-2 | s2cid = 16451230 }}</ref> The dorsal part of the ACC is connected with the [[prefrontal cortex]] and [[parietal cortex]], as well as the motor system and the [[frontal eye fields]],<ref name="Posner98">{{cite book |vauthors=Posner MI, DiGirolamo GJ |chapter=Executive attention: Conflict, target detection, and cognitive control |editor=Parasuraman R |title=The attentive brain |publisher=MIT Press |location=Cambridge, Mass |year=1998 |isbn=0-262-16172-9 }}</ref> making it a central station for processing [[Top-down and bottom-up design|top-down]] and [[Top-down and bottom-up design|bottom-up]] stimuli and assigning appropriate control to other areas in the brain. By contrast, the ventral part of the ACC is connected with the [[amygdala]], [[nucleus accumbens]], [[hypothalamus]], [[hippocampus]], and anterior [[insular cortex|insula]], and is involved in assessing the salience of emotion and motivational information. The ACC seems to be especially involved when effort is needed to carry out a task, such as in early learning and problem-solving.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Allman JM, Hakeem A, Erwin JM, Nimchinsky E, Hof P | title = The anterior cingulate cortex. The evolution of an interface between emotion and cognition | journal = Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences | volume = 935 | issue = 1 | pages = 107β17 | date = May 2001 | pmid = 11411161 | doi = 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb03476.x | bibcode = 2001NYASA.935..107A | s2cid = 10507342 }}</ref> On a cellular level, the ACC is unique in its abundance of specialized neurons called [[spindle cells]],<ref>{{cite book|last=Carter|first=Rita | name-list-style = vanc |title=The Human Brain Book|pages=124}}</ref> or [[von Economo neuron]]s. These cells are a relatively recent occurrence in evolutionary terms (found only in humans and other [[primates]], [[cetaceans]], and [[elephants]]) and contribute to this brain region's emphasis on addressing difficult problems, as well as the [[Spindle neuron#Related pathologies|pathologies]] related to the ACC.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Allman JM, Hakeem A, Erwin JM, Nimchinsky E, Hof P | title = The anterior cingulate cortex. The evolution of an interface between emotion and cognition | journal = Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences | volume = 935 | pages = 107β17 | date = May 2001 | issue = 1 | pmid = 11411161 | doi = 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb03476.x | bibcode = 2001NYASA.935..107A | s2cid = 10507342 }}</ref> ==Tasks== A typical task that activates the ACC involves eliciting some form of conflict within the participant that can potentially result in an error. One such task is called the [[Eriksen flanker task]] and consists of an arrow pointing to the left or right, which is flanked by two distractor arrows creating either compatible (<<<<<) or incompatible (>><>>) trials.<ref name="Botvin99">{{cite journal | vauthors = Botvinick M, Nystrom LE, Fissell K, Carter CS, Cohen JD | title = Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex | journal = Nature | volume = 402 | issue = 6758 | pages = 179β81 | date = November 1999 | pmid = 10647008 | doi = 10.1038/46035 | bibcode = 1999Natur.402..179B | s2cid = 4425726 }}</ref> Another very common conflict-inducing stimulus that activates the ACC is the [[Stroop task]], which involves naming the color ink of words that are either congruent ({{color|#ff0000|RED}} written in red) or incongruent ({{color|#0000ff|RED}} written in blue).<ref name="Pardo90">{{cite journal | vauthors = Pardo JV, Pardo PJ, Janer KW, Raichle ME | title = The anterior cingulate cortex mediates processing selection in the Stroop attentional conflict paradigm | journal = Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | volume = 87 | issue = 1 | pages = 256β9 | date = January 1990 | pmid = 2296583 | pmc = 53241 | doi = 10.1073/pnas.87.1.256 | bibcode = 1990PNAS...87..256P | doi-access = free }}</ref> Conflict occurs because people's reading abilities interfere with their attempt to correctly name the word's ink color. A variation of this task is the ''Counting-Stroop'', during which people count either neutral stimuli ('dog' presented four times) or interfering stimuli ('three' presented four times) by pressing a button. Another version of the ''Stroop task'' named the ''Emotional Counting Stroop'' is identical to the ''Counting Stroop'' test, except that it also uses segmented or repeated emotional words such as "murder" during the interference part of the task. Thus, ACC affects decision making of a task. ==Functions== Many studies attribute specific functions such as [[error detection]], anticipation of tasks, [[attention]],<ref name="Pardo90" /><ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Weissman DH, Gopalakrishnan A, Hazlett CJ, Woldorff MG | title = Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex resolves conflict from distracting stimuli by boosting attention toward relevant events | journal = Cerebral Cortex | volume = 15 | issue = 2 | pages = 229β37 | date = February 2005 | pmid = 15238434 | doi = 10.1093/cercor/bhh125 | doi-access = free }}</ref> [[motivation]], and modulation of emotional responses to the ACC.<ref name=Bush00/><ref name=Posner98/><ref name="Nieuw01">{{cite journal | vauthors = Nieuwenhuis S, Ridderinkhof KR, Blom J, Band GP, Kok A | title = Error-related brain potentials are differentially related to awareness of response errors: evidence from an antisaccade task | journal = Psychophysiology | volume = 38 | issue = 5 | pages = 752β60 | date = September 2001 | pmid = 11577898 | doi = 10.1111/1469-8986.3850752 | s2cid = 7566915 }}</ref> ===Error detection and conflict monitoring=== The most basic form of ACC theory states that the ACC is involved with [[error detection]].<ref name=Bush00/> Evidence has been derived from studies involving a [[Stroop task]].<ref name=Posner98/> However, ACC is also active during correct response, and this has been shown using a letter task, whereby participants had to respond to the letter X after an A was presented and ignore all other letter combinations with some letters more competitive than others.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Carter CS, Braver TS, Barch DM, Botvinick MM, Noll D, Cohen JD | title = Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance | journal = Science | volume = 280 | issue = 5364 | pages = 747β9 | date = May 1998 | pmid = 9563953 | doi = 10.1126/science.280.5364.747 | bibcode = 1998Sci...280..747C | s2cid = 264267292 }}</ref> They found that for more competitive stimuli ACC activation was greater. A similar theory poses that the ACC's primary function is the monitoring of conflict. In [[Eriksen flanker task]], incompatible trials produce the most conflict and the most activation by the ACC. Upon detection of a conflict, the ACC then provides cues to other areas in the brain to cope with the conflicting control systems. ==== Evidence from electrical studies ==== Evidence for ACC as having an error detection function comes from observations of [[error-related negativity]] (ERN) uniquely generated within the ACC upon error occurrences.<ref name=Bush00/><ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Gehring WJ, Goss B, Coles MG, Meyer DE, Donchin E |title=A neural system for error-detection and compensation |journal=Psychological Science |volume=4 |issue=6 |pages=385β90 |date=November 1993 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00586.x |s2cid=17422146 }}</ref><ref name="Holroyd04">{{cite book |vauthors=Holroyd CB, Nieuwenhuis S, Mars RB, Coles MG |chapter=Anterior cingulate cortex, selection for action, and error processing |editor=Posner MI |title=Cognitive neuroscience of attention |publisher=Guilford Press |location=New York |year=2004 |pages=[https://archive.org/details/cognitiveneurosc0000unse_q4e4/page/219 219β31] |isbn=1-59385-048-4 |chapter-url=https://archive.org/details/cognitiveneurosc0000unse_q4e4/page/219 }}</ref><ref name="Luu04">{{cite book |vauthors=Luu P, Pederson SM |chapter=The anterior cingulate cortex: Regulating actions in context |editor=Posner MI |title=Cognitive neuroscience of attention |publisher=Guilford Press |location=New York |year=2004 |isbn=1-59385-048-4 |url-access=registration |url=https://archive.org/details/cognitiveneurosc0000unse_q4e4 }}</ref> A distinction has been made between an [[Event-related potential|ERP]] following incorrect responses (response ERN) and a signal after subjects receive feedback after erroneous responses (feedback ERN). Patients with lateral prefrontal cingulate (PFC) damage show reduced ERNs.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Gehring WJ, Knight RT | title = Prefrontal-cingulate interactions in action monitoring | journal = Nature Neuroscience | volume = 3 | issue = 5 | pages = 516β20 | date = May 2000 | pmid = 10769394 | doi = 10.1038/74899 | s2cid = 11136447 }}</ref> Reinforcement learning ERN theory poses that there is a mismatch between actual response execution and appropriate response execution, which results in an ERN discharge.<ref name=Bush00/><ref name=Holroyd04/> Furthermore, this theory predicts that, when the ACC receives conflicting input from control areas in the brain, it determines and allocates which area should be given control over the motor system. Varying levels of dopamine are believed to influence the optimization of this filter system by providing expectations about the outcomes of an event. The ERN, then, serves as a beacon to highlight the violation of an expectation.<ref name=Luu04/> Research on the occurrence of the feedback ERN shows evidence that this potential has larger amplitudes when violations of expectancy are large. In other words, if an event is not likely to happen, the feedback ERN will be larger if no error is detected. Other studies have examined whether the ERN is elicited by varying the cost of an error and the evaluation of a response.<ref name=Holroyd04/> In these trials, feedback is given about whether the participant has gained or lost money after a response. Amplitudes of ERN responses with small gains and small losses were similar. No ERN was elicited for any losses as opposed to an ERN for no wins, even though both outcomes are the same. The finding in this paradigm suggests that monitoring for wins and losses is based on the relative expected gains and losses. If you get a different outcome than expected, the ERN will be larger than for expected outcomes. ERN studies have also localized specific functions of the ACC.<ref name=Luu04/> The rostral ACC seems to be active after an error commission, indicating an error response function, whereas the dorsal ACC is active after both an error and feedback, suggesting a more evaluative function (for fMRI evidence, see also<ref name="Bush02">{{cite journal | vauthors = Bush G, Vogt BA, Holmes J, Dale AM, Greve D, Jenike MA, Rosen BR | title = Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex: a role in reward-based decision making | journal = Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | volume = 99 | issue = 1 | pages = 523β8 | date = January 2002 | pmid = 11756669 | pmc = 117593 | doi = 10.1073/pnas.012470999 | bibcode = 2002PNAS...99..523B | doi-access = free }}</ref><ref name="Polli">{{cite journal | vauthors = Polli FE, Barton JJ, Cain MS, Thakkar KN, Rauch SL, Manoach DS | title = Rostral and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex make dissociable contributions during antisaccade error commission | journal = Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | volume = 102 | issue = 43 | pages = 15700β5 | date = October 2005 | pmid = 16227444 | pmc = 1255733 | doi = 10.1073/pnas.0503657102 | bibcode = 2005PNAS..10215700P | doi-access = free }}</ref><ref name="Taylor06">{{cite journal | vauthors = Taylor SF, Martis B, Fitzgerald KD, Welsh RC, Abelson JL, Liberzon I, Himle JA, Gehring WJ | title = Medial frontal cortex activity and loss-related responses to errors | journal = The Journal of Neuroscience | volume = 26 | issue = 15 | pages = 4063β70 | date = April 2006 | pmid = 16611823 | pmc = 6673891 | doi = 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4709-05.2006 }}</ref> ). This evaluation is emotional in nature and highlights the amount of distress associated with a certain error.<ref name=Bush00/> Summarizing the evidence found by ERN studies, it appears to be the case that ACC receives information about a stimulus, selects an appropriate response, monitors the action, and adapts behavior if there is a violation of expectancy.<ref name=Luu04/> === Social evaluation === Activity in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) has been implicated in processing both the detection and appraisal of social processes, including [[social exclusion]]. When exposed to repeated personal social evaluative tasks, non-depressed women showed reduced [[fMRI]] [[Blood-oxygen-level dependent imaging|BOLD]] activation in the dACC on the second exposure, while women with a history of [[Depression (mood)|depression]] exhibited enhanced BOLD activation. This differential activity may reflect enhanced rumination about social evaluation or enhanced arousal associated with repeated social evaluation.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Dedovic K, Slavich GM, Muscatell KA, Irwin MR, Eisenberger NI | title = Dorsal Anterior Cingulate Cortex Responses to Repeated Social Evaluative Feedback in Young Women with and without a History of Depression | journal = Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience | volume = 10 | pages = 64 | date = 2016 | pmid = 27065828 | pmc = 4815251 | doi = 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00064 | doi-access = free }}</ref> The anterior cingulate cortex gyrus is involved in effort to help others.<ref>{{cite web|url=https://neurosciencenews.com/accg-helping-hand-21297/|title=When Can We Be Bothered to Help Others? Brain Region Responsible for This Behavior Discovered|date=August 26, 2022}}</ref> ===Reward-based learning theory=== A more comprehensive and recent theory describes the ACC as a more active component and poses that it detects and monitors errors, evaluates the degree of the error, and then suggests an appropriate form of action to be implemented by the motor system. Earlier evidence from electrical studies indicate the ACC has an evaluative component, which is indeed confirmed by [[fMRI]] studies. The dorsal and rostral areas of the ACC both seem to be affected by rewards and losses associated with errors. During one study, participants received monetary rewards and losses for correct and incorrect responses, respectively.<ref name=Bush02/> Largest activation in the dACC was shown during loss trials. This stimulus did not elicit any errors, and, thus, error detection and monitoring theories cannot fully explain why this ACC activation would occur. The dorsal part of the ACC seems to play a key role in reward-based decision-making and learning. The rostral part of the ACC, on the other hand, is believed to be involved more with affective responses to errors. In an interesting expansion of the previously described experiment, the effects of rewards and costs on ACC's activation during error commission was examined.<ref name=Taylor06/> Participants performed a version of the [[Eriksen flanker task]] using a set of letters assigned to each response button instead of arrows. Targets were flanked by either a congruent or an incongruent set of letters. Using an image of a thumb (up, down, or neutral), participants received feedback on how much money they gained or lost. The researchers found greater rostral ACC activation when participants lost money during the trials. The participants reported being frustrated when making mistakes. Because the ACC is intricately involved with error detection and affective responses, it may very well be that this area forms the basis of self-confidence. Taken together, these findings indicate that both the dorsal and rostral areas are involved in evaluating the extent of the error and optimizing subsequent responses. A study confirming this notion explored the functions of both the dorsal and rostral areas of the ACC involved using a saccade task.<ref name=Polli/> Participants were shown a cue that indicated whether they had to make either a pro-saccade or an anti-saccade. An anti-saccade requires suppression of a distracting cue because the target appears in the opposite location causing the conflict. Results showed differing activation for the rostral and dorsal ACC areas. Early correct anti-saccade performance was associated with rostral activation. The dorsal area, on the other hand, was activated when errors were committed, but also for correct responses. Whenever the dorsal area was active, fewer errors were committed providing more evidence that the ACC is involved with [[Effortfulness|effortful]] performance. The second finding showed that, during error trials, the ACC activated later than for correct responses, clearly indicating a kind of evaluative function. ===Role in consciousness=== The ACC area in the brain is associated with many functions that are correlated with conscious experience. Greater ACC activation levels were present in more emotionally aware female participants when shown short 'emotional' video clips.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Lane RD, Reiman EM, Axelrod B, Yun LS, Holmes A, Schwartz GE | title = Neural correlates of levels of emotional awareness. Evidence of an interaction between emotion and attention in the anterior cingulate cortex | journal = Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | volume = 10 | issue = 4 | pages = 525β35 | date = July 1998 | pmid = 9712681 | doi = 10.1162/089892998562924 | s2cid = 27743177 }}</ref> Better emotional awareness is associated with improved recognition of emotional cues or targets, which is reflected by ACC activation. The idea of awareness being associated with the ACC is supported by some evidence, in that it seems to be the case that, when subjects' responses are not congruent with actual responses, a larger [[error-related negativity]] is produced.<ref name=Luu04/> One study found an ERN even when subjects were not aware of their error.<ref name=Luu04/> Awareness may not be necessary to elicit an ERN, but it could influence the effect of the amplitude of the feedback ERN. Relating to the reward-based learning theory, awareness could modulate expectancy violations. Increased awareness could result in decreased violations of expectancies and decreased awareness could achieve the opposite effect. Further research is needed to completely understand the effects of awareness on ACC activation. In ''[[The Astonishing Hypothesis]]'', [[Francis Crick]] identifies the anterior cingulate, to be specific the anterior cingulate sulcus, as a likely candidate for the center of [[free will]] in humans. Crick bases this suggestion on scans of patients with specific lesions that seem to interfere with their sense of independent will, such as [[alien hand syndrome]]. ===Role in registering pain=== The ACC registers physical pain as shown in functional MRI studies that showed an increase in signal intensity, typically in the posterior part of area 24 of the ACC, that was correlated with pain intensity. When this pain-related activation was accompanied by attention-demanding cognitive tasks (verbal fluency), the attention-demanding tasks increased signal intensity in a region of the ACC anterior and/or superior to the pain-related activation region.<ref>Davis, Karen D., Stephen J. Taylor, Adrian P. Crawley, Michael L. Wood, and David J. Mikulis. "Functional MRI of pain- and attention-related activations in the human cingulate cortex", ''J. Neurophysiol.'' volume 77: pages 3370β3380, 1997 [http://jn.physiology.org/content/77/6/3370.short]</ref> The ACC is the cortical area that has been most frequently linked to the experience of pain.<ref>{{cite book|last=Pinel|first=John P.J. | name-list-style = vanc |title= Biopsychology |year=2011 |publisher=Allyn & Bacon |location=Boston|isbn=978-0-205-83256-9|page=181|edition=8th }}</ref> It appears to be involved in the emotional reaction to pain rather than to the perception of pain itself.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Price DD | s2cid = 15250446 | title = Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affective dimension of pain | journal = Science | volume = 288 | issue = 5472 | pages = 1769β72 | date = June 2000 | pmid = 10846154 | doi = 10.1126/science.288.5472.1769 | bibcode = 2000Sci...288.1769P }}</ref> Evidence from social neuroscience studies have suggested that, in addition to its role in physical pain, the ACC may also be involved in monitoring painful social situations as well, such as exclusion or rejection. When participants felt socially excluded in an fMRI virtual ball throwing game in which the ball was never thrown to the participant, the ACC showed activation. Further, this activation was correlated with a self-reported measure of social distress, indicating that the ACC may be involved in the detection and monitoring of social situations which may cause social/emotional pain, rather than just physical pain.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Eisenberger NI, Lieberman MD, Williams KD | title = Does rejection hurt? An FMRI study of social exclusion | journal = Science | volume = 302 | issue = 5643 | pages = 290β2 | date = October 2003 | pmid = 14551436 | doi = 10.1126/science.1089134 | bibcode = 2003Sci...302..290E | s2cid = 21253445 }}</ref> ==Pathology== Studying the effects of damage to the ACC provides insights into the type of functions it serves in the intact brain. Behavior that is associated with lesions in the ACC includes: inability to detect errors, severe difficulty with resolving stimulus conflict in a [[Stroop task]], emotional instability, inattention, and [[akinetic mutism]].<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Janer KW, Pardo JV | title = Deficits in selective attention following bilateral anterior cingulotomy | journal = Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | volume = 3 | issue = 3 | pages = 231β41 | date = 1991 | pmid = 23964838 | doi = 10.1162/jocn.1991.3.3.231 | s2cid = 39599951 }}</ref><ref name=Bush00/><ref name=Posner98/> There is evidence that damage to ACC is present in patients with [[schizophrenia]], where studies have shown patients have difficulty in dealing with conflicting spatial locations in a Stroop-like task and having abnormal ERNs.<ref name=Posner98/><ref name=Holroyd04/> Participants with [[ADHD]] were found to have reduced activation in the dorsal area of the ACC when performing the [[Stroop task]].<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Bush G, Frazier JA, Rauch SL, Seidman LJ, Whalen PJ, Jenike MA, Rosen BR, Biederman J | title = Anterior cingulate cortex dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder revealed by fMRI and the Counting Stroop | journal = Biological Psychiatry | volume = 45 | issue = 12 | pages = 1542β52 | date = June 1999 | pmid = 10376114 | doi = 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00083-9 | s2cid = 205870638 }}</ref> Together, these findings corroborate results from imaging and electrical studies about the variety of functions attributed to the ACC. ===OCD=== There is strong evidence that this area may have a role in [[obsessiveβcompulsive disorder]]. A recent study from the University of Cambridge showed that participants with OCD had higher levels of [[Glutamate (neurotransmitter)|glutamate]] and lower levels of [[Ξ-Aminobutyric acid|GABA]] in the anterior cingulate cortex, compared to participants without OCD. They used magnetic resonance spectroscopy to assess the balance of excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmission by measuring glutamate and GABA levels in anterior cingulate cortex and supplementary motor area of healthy volunteers and participants with OCD. Participants with OCD had significantly higher levels of glutamate and lower levels of GABA in the ACC and a higher Glu:GABA ratio in that region.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Biria |first1=Marjan |last2=Banca |first2=Paula |last3=Healy |first3=MΓ‘irΓ©ad P. |last4=Keser |first4=Engin |last5=Sawiak |first5=Stephen J. |last6=Rodgers |first6=Christopher T. |last7=Rua |first7=Catarina |last8=de Souza |first8=Ana Maria Frota LisbΓ΄a Pereira |last9=Marzuki |first9=Aleya A. |last10=Sule |first10=Akeem |last11=Ersche |first11=Karen D. |last12=Robbins |first12=Trevor W. |date=2023-06-27 |title=Cortical glutamate and GABA are related to compulsive behaviour in individuals with obsessive compulsive disorder and healthy controls |journal=Nature Communications |language=en |volume=14 |issue=1 |page=3324 |doi=10.1038/s41467-023-38695-z |pmid=37369695 |issn=2041-1723|pmc=10300066 |bibcode=2023NatCo..14.3324B }}</ref> Recent [[Seed-based d mapping|SDM meta-analyses]] of [[voxel-based morphometry]] studies comparing people with OCD and healthy controls has found people with OCD to have increased [[grey matter]] volumes in bilateral [[lenticular nucleus|lenticular nuclei]], extending to the [[caudate nucleus|caudate nuclei]], while decreased grey matter volumes in bilateral dorsal [[medial frontal gyrus|medial frontal]]/anterior cingulate cortex.<ref name="Radua and Mataix-Cols, 2009">{{cite journal | vauthors = Radua J, Mataix-Cols D | title = Voxel-wise meta-analysis of grey matter changes in obsessive-compulsive disorder | journal = The British Journal of Psychiatry | volume = 195 | issue = 5 | pages = 393β402 | date = November 2009 | pmid = 19880927 | doi = 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.055046 | doi-access = free }}</ref><ref name="radua2010">{{cite journal | vauthors = Radua J, van den Heuvel OA, Surguladze S, Mataix-Cols D | title = Meta-analytical comparison of voxel-based morphometry studies in obsessive-compulsive disorder vs other anxiety disorders | journal = Archives of General Psychiatry | volume = 67 | issue = 7 | pages = 701β11 | date = July 2010 | pmid = 20603451 | doi = 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.70 | doi-access = free }} </ref> These findings contrast with those in people with other anxiety disorders, who evince decreased (rather than increased) [[grey matter]] volumes in bilateral [[lenticular nucleus|lenticular]] / [[caudate nucleus|caudate nuclei]], while also decreased grey matter volumes in bilateral dorsal [[medial frontal gyrus|medial frontal]] / anterior cingulate gyri.<ref name="radua2010" /> ===Schizophrenia spectrum disorders=== In individuals with [[schizophrenia]] spectrum disorders, the anterior cingulate cortex has been found to be smaller compared to that of control subjects.<ref name="Haznedar 2004">{{Cite journal | last1 = Haznedar | first1 = M. M. | last2 = Buchsbaum | first2 = M. S. | last3 = Hazlett | first3 = E. A. | last4 = Shihabuddin | first4 = L. | last5 = New | first5 = A. | last6 = Siever | first6 = L. J. | doi = 10.1016/j.schres.2004.02.025 | title = Cingulate gyrus volume and metabolism in the schizophrenia spectrum | journal = Schizophrenia Research | volume = 71 | issue = 2β3 | pages = 249β262 | year = 2004 | pmid = 15474896| s2cid = 28889346 }}</ref><ref name="Fujiwara 2007">{{Cite journal | last1 = Fujiwara | first1 = H. | last2 = Hirao | first2 = K. | last3 = Namiki | first3 = C. | last4 = Yamada | first4 = M. | last5 = Shimizu | first5 = M. | last6 = Fukuyama | first6 = H. | last7 = Hayashi | first7 = T. | last8 = Murai | first8 = T. | doi = 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.068 | title = Anterior cingulate pathology and social cognition in schizophrenia: A study of gray matter, white matter and sulcal morphometry | journal = NeuroImage | volume = 36 | issue = 4 | pages = 1236β1245 | year = 2007 | pmid = 17524666| url = https://repository.kulib.kyoto-u.ac.jp/dspace/bitstream/2433/124279/3/yigak03320.pdf | hdl = 2433/124279 | s2cid = 25750603 | hdl-access = free }}</ref> [[Meta-analyses]] have shown that its activity is reduced during emotion processing,<ref>{{cite journal |last1= Taylor |first1=S-F |last2= Kang |first2=J |last3= Brege |first3=I-S |last4= Tso |first4=I-F | last5= Hosanagar |first5=A | last6= Johnson |first6=T | title= Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of emotion perception and experience in schizophrenia |journal=Biological Psychiatry |date=15 Jan 2012 |volume=71| issue=2| pages=136-145| doi=10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.09.007|pmid= 21993193}}</ref> and its [[functional connectivity]] with the [[striatum]] is diminished at rest, which has been linked to cognitive rigidity.<ref>{{cite journal |last1= Grimaldi |first1=D-A |last2= Patane' |first2=A |last3= Cattarinussi |first3=G |last4= Sambataro |first4=F |title= Functional connectivity of the striatum in psychosis: Meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies and replication on an independent sample |journal=Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews |date=2 May 2025 |volume=174 |doi=10.1016/j.neubiorev.2025.106179 |pmid= 40288705}}</ref> ===Anxiety=== The ACC has been suggested to have possible links with [[Social Anxiety|social anxiety]], along with the amygdala part of the brain, but this research is still in its early stages.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Lieberman MD, Eisenberger NI | title = Neuroscience. Pains and pleasures of social life | journal = Science | volume = 323 | issue = 5916 | pages = 890β1 | date = February 2009 | pmid = 19213907 | doi = 10.1126/science.1170008 | s2cid = 206518219 }}</ref> A more recent study, by the Wake Forest Baptist Medical Centre, confirms the relationship between the ACC and anxiety regulation, by revealing mindfulness practice as a meditator for anxiety precisely through the ACC.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Zeidan F, Martucci KT, Kraft RA, McHaffie JG, Coghill RC | title = Neural correlates of mindfulness meditation-related anxiety relief | journal = Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience | volume = 9 | issue = 6 | pages = 751β9 | date = June 2014 | pmid = 23615765 | pmc = 4040088 | doi = 10.1093/scan/nst041 }}</ref> ===Depression=== The adjacent [[subgenual area|subcallosal cingulate gyrus]] has been implicated in [[major depression]] and research indicates that [[deep-brain stimulation]] of the region could act to alleviate depressive symptoms.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Hamani C, Mayberg H, Stone S, Laxton A, Haber S, Lozano AM | title = The subcallosal cingulate gyrus in the context of major depression | journal = Biological Psychiatry | volume = 69 | issue = 4 | pages = 301β8 | date = February 2011 | pmid = 21145043 | doi = 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.034 | s2cid = 35458273 | doi-access = free }}</ref> Although people with depression had smaller subgenual ACCs,<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = OngΓΌr D, Ferry AT, Price JL | title = Architectonic subdivision of the human orbital and medial prefrontal cortex | journal = The Journal of Comparative Neurology | volume = 460 | issue = 3 | pages = 425β49 | date = June 2003 | pmid = 12692859 | doi = 10.1002/cne.10609 | s2cid = 9798173 }}</ref> their ACCs were more active when adjusted for size. This correlates well with increased subgenual ACC activity during sadness in healthy people,<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = George MS, Ketter TA, Parekh PI, Horwitz B, Herscovitch P, Post RM | title = Brain activity during transient sadness and happiness in healthy women | journal = The American Journal of Psychiatry | volume = 152 | issue = 3 | pages = 341β51 | date = March 1995 | pmid = 7864258 | doi = 10.1176/ajp.152.3.341 }}</ref> and normalization of activity after successful treatment.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Licinio |first1=Julio|last2=Wong|first2=Ma-Li | name-list-style = vanc |title=Biology of Depression: From Novel Insights to Therapeutic Strategies|url=https://archive.org/details/biologydepressio00lici |url-access=limited |date=29 January 2008|publisher=Wiley-VCH Verlag GmbH & Co. KGaA|isbn=9783527307852|pages=[https://archive.org/details/biologydepressio00lici/page/n455 425]β466}}</ref> Of note, the activity of the subgenual cingulate cortex correlates with individual differences in negative affect during the baseline resting state; in other words, the greater the subgenual activity, the greater the negative affectivity in temperament.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Zald DH, Mattson DL, Pardo JV | title = Brain activity in ventromedial prefrontal cortex correlates with individual differences in negative affect | journal = Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | volume = 99 | issue = 4 | pages = 2450β4 | date = February 2002 | pmid = 11842195 | pmc = 122385 | doi = 10.1073/pnas.042457199 | bibcode = 2002PNAS...99.2450Z | doi-access = free }}</ref> ===Lead exposure=== A study of brain MRIs taken on adults that had previously participated in the [[Cincinnati Lead Study]] found that people that had higher levels of [[lead]] exposure as children had decreased brain size as adults. This effect was most pronounced in the ACC (Cecil et al., 2008)<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Cecil KM, Brubaker CJ, Adler CM, Dietrich KN, Altaye M, Egelhoff JC, Wessel S, Elangovan I, Hornung R, Jarvis K, Lanphear BP | title = Decreased brain volume in adults with childhood lead exposure | journal = PLOS Medicine | volume = 5 | issue = 5 | pages = e112 | date = May 2008 | pmid = 18507499 | pmc = 2689675 | doi = 10.1371/journal.pmed.0050112 | doi-access = free }}</ref> and is thought to relate to the cognitive and behavioral deficits of affected individuals. ===Autism=== Impairments in the development of the anterior cingulate, together with impairments in the dorsal medial-frontal cortex, may constitute a neural substrate for socio-cognitive deficits in [[autism spectrum disorders|autism]], such as social orienting and [[joint attention]].<ref>{{cite journal|url=http://infantlab.fiu.edu/articles/Mundy,%202003%20JCPP.pdf |title=Annotation: The neural basis of social impairments in autism: the role of the dorsal medial-frontal cortex and anterior cingulate system |author=Peter Mundy |journal=Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry |year=2003 |volume=44 |issue=6 |pages=793β809 |doi=10.1111/1469-7610.00165 |pmid=12959489 |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120307115650/http://infantlab.fiu.edu/articles/Mundy%2C%202003%20JCPP.pdf |archive-date=7 March 2012 }}</ref> ===PTSD=== An increasing number of studies are investigating the role of the ACC in [[post-traumatic stress disorder]]. PTSD diagnosis and related symptoms such as skin conductance response (SCR) to "potentially startling sounds" were found to be correlated with reduced ACC volume.<ref>{{cite journal | vauthors = Young DA, Chao L, Neylan TC, O'Donovan A, Metzler TJ, Inslicht SS | title = Association among anterior cingulate cortex volume, psychophysiological response, and PTSD diagnosis in a Veteran sample | journal = Neurobiology of Learning and Memory | volume = 155 | pages = 189β196 | date = November 2018 | pmid = 30086395 | pmc = 6361720 | doi = 10.1016/j.nlm.2018.08.006 }}</ref> Further, childhood trauma and executive dysfunction seem to correlate with reduced ACC connectivity to surrounding neural regions.<ref name="Silveira 499β509">{{cite journal | vauthors = Silveira S, Shah R, Nooner KB, Nagel BJ, Tapert SF, de Bellis MD, Mishra J | title = Impact of Childhood Trauma on Executive Function in Adolescence-Mediating Functional Brain Networks and Prediction of High-Risk Drinking | journal = Biological Psychiatry. Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging | volume = 5 | issue = 5 | pages = 499β509 | date = May 2020 | pmid = 32299789 | doi = 10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.01.011 | pmc = 8366521 }}</ref> In a longitudinal study, this reduced connectivity was able to predict high-risk drinking (binge drinking at least once per week for the past 12 months) up to four years later.<ref name="Silveira 499β509"/> ===General risk of psychopathology=== A study on differences in brain structure of adults with high and low levels of [[Metacognitive therapy#Self-Regulatory Executive Function model|cognitive-attentional syndrome]] demonstrated diminished volume of the dorsal part of the ACC in the former group, indicating relationship between cortical thickness of ACC and general risk of [[psychopathology]].<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Kowalski |first1=Joachim |last2=Wypych |first2=Marek |last3=Marchewka |first3=Artur |last4=Dragan |first4=MaΕgorzata |date=2022-03-10 |title=Brain structural correlates of cognitive-attentional syndrome β a Voxel-Based Morphometry Study |url=https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-022-00649-2 |journal=Brain Imaging and Behavior |volume=16 |issue=4 |pages=1914β1918 |language=en |doi=10.1007/s11682-022-00649-2 |pmid=35266100 |s2cid=247360689 |issn=1931-7565|url-access=subscription }}</ref> ==Additional images== <gallery> Image:Medial surface of cerebral cortex - gyri.png|Medial surface of human cerebral cortex - gyri Image:Anterior Cingulate Cortex.png|Anterior Cingulate Cortex of monkey ([[Macaca mulatta]]). File:Caudal Anterior Cingulate - DK ATLAS.png| Caudal Anterior Cingulate gyrus File:Rostral Anterior Cingulate - DK ATLAS.png | Rostral Anterior Cingulate gyrus </gallery> == See also == {{Commons category|Anterior cingulate cortex}} * [[Cingulate cortex]] * [[Cingulate gyrus]] * [[Cingulate sulcus]] * [[subgenual cingulate cortex]] * [[subcallosal cortex]] * [[Reward system]] == References == {{reflist|30em}} {{Prosencephalon}} [[Category:Cerebral cortex]] [[Category:Medial surface of cerebral hemisphere]]
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Pages transcluded onto the current version of this page
(
help
)
:
Template:Cite book
(
edit
)
Template:Cite journal
(
edit
)
Template:Cite web
(
edit
)
Template:Color
(
edit
)
Template:Commons category
(
edit
)
Template:Ifsubst
(
edit
)
Template:Infobox brain
(
edit
)
Template:Prosencephalon
(
edit
)
Template:Reflist
(
edit
)
Template:Short description
(
edit
)
Template:Sister project
(
edit
)
Template:Use dmy dates
(
edit
)