Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Case role
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
'''Case roles''', according to the work by [[Charles J. Fillmore]] (1967),<ref name=Fillmore>{{cite journal|last=Fillmore|first=Charles J.|title=The case for case|journal=Proceedings of the Texas Symposium, on Language Universals|date=April 1967}}</ref> are the [[semantics|semantic]] roles of [[noun]] phrases (NP) in relation to the syntactic structures that contain these noun phrases. The term ''case role'' is most widely used for purely semantic relations, including [[theta role]]s and [[thematic relations|thematic roles]], that can be independent of the [[morpho-syntax]]. The concept of case roles is related to the larger notion of ''Case'' (with a capitalised C), which is defined as a system of marking dependent nouns for the type of semantic or syntactic relationship they bear to their heads. Case traditionally refers to [[inflection]]al marking.<ref>{{cite book|last=Blake|first=Barry J.|title=Case|year=2001|publisher=Cambridge University Press|location=Port Chester, NY, USA|isbn=9780511154249|url=http://www.cambridge.org}}</ref> The relationships between nouns and their containing structures are of both [[syntax|syntactic]] and semantic value. The syntactic positional relationships between forms in sentences vary cross-linguistically and allows [[Linguist|grammarians]] to observe semantic values in these nouns by examining their syntactic values. Using these semantic values gives the base for considering case roles in a specific language.<ref name="Fillmore"/> In addition to its inventory of structural cases, case theory includes a series of lexical cases that are assigned at deep structure in conjunction with [[theta role]] assignment.<ref name=Woolford>{{cite journal|last=Woolford|first=Ellen|title=Four-way case systems: Ergative, nominative, objective and accusative|journal=Natural Language and Linguistic Theory|year=1997|volume=15|issue=1|pages=181–227|doi=10.1023/A:1005796113097|s2cid=169330717 }}</ref> In addition to its relation to Case (case based on syntactic structures), these semantic notions of case role are also closely related to morphological case.<ref name="Fillmore"/> ==Inventory of case roles== The following list of case roles is frequently distinguished in literature but is not an exhaustive list, as there is no consensus on the [[Linguistic universal|universal]] inventory of roles, nor a universal agreement as to the correct assignment of [[Constituent (linguistics)|constituents]] to roles.<ref>{{cite book|last=Blake|first=Barry J.|title=Case|year=1994|publisher=Cambridge University Press|location=Cambridge [England]|isbn=0-521-44114-5|pages=67–71|edition=1. publ., repr.}}</ref> {| class="wikitable" |- ! Case Role !! Description !! Example |- | [[Patient (grammar)|Patient]] || [[Charles J. Fillmore|Fillmore]] refers to this case role as 'object' or 'objective' and can be described in three ways: # an entity viewed as existing in a state or undergoing change # an entity viewed as located or moving # an entity viewed as affected by an entity || <br />1. '''The sky''' is blue.<br />2. '''The lion''' is in the cave.<br />3. The bird ate '''the worm'''. |- | [[Agent (grammar)|Agent]] || An entity that performs an activity or brings about a change of state || '''The robots''' assembled the car. |- | Instrument || The means by which an activity or change of state is carried out || She squashed the spider with '''a slipper'''. |- | Experiencer || The creature experiencing an emotion or perception || '''They''' love music. |- | [[locative case|Location]] || The position of an entity, referring to both the temporal and spatial roles || The vase is on '''the table'''. <br />or <br />Canada Day is on '''a Tuesday'''. |- | Source || The point from which an entity moves or derives || They got news from '''home'''. |- | Destination || The point to or towards which an entity moves or is oriented || He turned to the '''altar''' and walked towards '''it'''. |- | Recipient || A sentient destination || She gave her spare change to '''the collectors'''. |- | Purpose || The purpose of an activity || He went to the Red Rooster for '''some take-out'''. |- | Beneficiary || The animate entity on whose behalf an activity is carried out || She did the shopping for '''her mother'''. |- | Manner || The way in which an activity is done or the way in which a change of state takes place || He did it with '''great skill'''. |- | Extent || The distance, area or time over which an activity is carried out or over which a state holds || It lasted '''the winter'''. |- | Possessor || The entity that possesses another entity || I saw '''John's''' golf clubs. |} ==Early contributions to case role== === Roman Jakobson's work on case roles in Russian === In his article on the case system of [[Russian language|Russian]], [[Roman Jakobson]] (1958) closely examines case assignment and argues for a feature decomposition of case on the basis of semantic considerations.<ref name="Jakobson">{{cite journal|last=Jakobson|first=R.O.|title=Contribution to the General Theory of Case: General Meanings of the Russian Cases|journal=Gesamtbedeutung der Russischen Kasus: Beitrag zur Allgemeinen Kasuslehre|series=In L.R. Waugh & H. Morris (eds.)|pages=59–103}}</ref> Jakobson (1958) proposed a three-feature binary case system for Russian case which includes the following: ±marginal, ±quantifying, and ±ascriptive, where the negative value is considered to be unmarked{{Clarification needed|reason=What does 'markedness' mean in this context?|date=July 2024}}. The term ''marginal'' distinguishes the direct and non-direct cases; only the -marginal cases may occur in subject and object position. [[Quantification (linguistics)|''Quantifying'']] indicates the relevance of the extent to which the noun is a participant in the event. ''Ascriptive'' puts emphasis on directionality.<ref name="Jakobson" /> Jakobson considers cases to be bundles of these three features, which can be assigned to morphological cases: {| class="wikitable" |- ! Case !! Marginal!! Quantifying !! Ascriptive |- |[[Nominative]]|| :- || ::- || :- |- |[[Accusative case|Accusative]]|| :- || ::- || :+ |- |[[Genitive case|Genitive 1]]|| :- || ::+ || :+ |- |Genitive 2|| :- || ::+ || :- |- |[[Locative case|Locative 1]]|| :+ || ::+ || :+ |- |Locative 2|| :+ || ::+ || :- |- |[[Dative case|Dative]]|| :+ || ::- || :+ |- |[[Instrumental case|Instrumental]]|| :+ || ::- || :- |} He uses these decomposed case features to account for the case alternations in subject and object position, and argues for how there should be a universal inventory of case roles.<ref name="Jakobson"/> ==Multiple case roles== There is a theory that multiple case roles can be assigned to noun phrases.<ref name=Bhat>{{cite journal|last=Bhat|first=D. N. S.|title=Multiple case roles|journal=Lingua|year=1997|volume=42|issue=4|pages=365–377|doi=10.1016/0024-3841(77)90105-X}}</ref> The reasons for having more than one case role is due to the differences in the sentences’ semantic effects.<ref name="Bhat"/> Bhat (1997) proposed that the speaker of a language would have the option of assigning any single case role out of the multiple case role alternations available in a given context.<ref name="Bhat"/> This is not done by a [[transformational grammar|transformational rule]], but due to the [[transformational grammar|deep structure representations]] (the core semantic relations of the sentence).<ref name="Bhat"/> Examples (1a) || ''John sprayed the wall with paint.'' (1b) || ''John sprayed paint on the wall.'' In comparing sentences (1a) and (1b), it demonstrates that the [[transformational grammar|surface structure representation]] for each sentence (the structure following the [[Phonology|phonological]] form of the sentence) is different.<ref name="Bhat"/> However, in order to account for the meaning distinctions that exist in (1a) and (1b), it has been demonstrated that these distinctions are due to the differences in the deep structures of each sentence and can be resolved by assigning a ''different'' case role to the NP. With 'paint' acting as an instrument, and 'the wall' being the location, sentence (1a) might infer that all the paint was used, but that not necessarily all the wall was covered. Sentence (1b) might imply that the whole wall is covered, but that John did not use all the paint he had available to him.<ref name="Bhat"/> === Fillmore's alternative theory === Conversely, Fillmore (1968) suggested that in the case of sentences following the structure of the above examples, the noun phrases are not assigned multiple case roles, but instead retain the same case roles in both sentences (instrument for 'paint', and location for 'wall'). The difference in meaning is attributed to a transformation that takes both identical deep structures and chooses the direct object as it appears in the surface form.<ref name="Fillmore 1968">{{cite journal |last=Fillmore |first=Charles, J. |year=1968 |title=The case for case |journal=Universals in Linguistic Theory |pages=1–88}}</ref> === Examples in Kannada === [[Kannada Language|Kannada]] (a language spoken in [[India]] with overt usage of case in its suffixes) affords some good evidence of how multiple case roles can be assigned to NPs in the following two examples: (1) how NPs can be assigned either [[Object (grammar)|Object]] or [[Locative case|Location]] case roles, and (2) how NPs can be assigned either [[Agent (grammar)|Agent]] or [[Thematic relation|Experiencer]] case roles.<ref name="Bhat"/> Evidence for multiple case roles demonstrated in Kannada:<ref name=Bhat/> NPs can be assigned either ''object'' or ''location'' case roles. Evidence is found from the meaning distinctions of exhaustiveness: {{interlinear|lang=kn|number=(2a) | bekku maravannu hattitu | cat(NOM) tree(ACC) climbed | "The cat climbed the tree."}} {{interlinear|lang=kn|number=(2b) | bekku marakke hattitu | cat(NOM) tree(DAT) climbed | "The cat climbed to the tree."}} (2a) implies that the cat climbed the tree from the ground itself, whereas (2b) has no such implication. The common feature of these two uses is that whenever an element occurs as the ''object'' case role, it gets the added meaning of being exhaustively affected by the action denoted by the [[verb]] as seen in (2a) (like climbing up the tree completely from the ground upwards). No such additional meaning is observed in sentences in which the element has been used as a ''Location'' case role (as in 2b). How NPs can be assigned either ''agent'' or ''experiencer'' case roles: Evidence found from the meaning distinctions of [[Volition (psychology)|volition]]. {{interlinear|lang=kn|number=(3a) | ra:ju pa:thavannu maretidda:ne | Raju(NOM) lesson(ACC) has-forgotten | "Raju has forgotten the lesson."}} {{interlinear|lang=kn|number=(3b) | ra:juvige pa:tha maretide | Raju(DAT) lesson(NOM) forgotten | "The lesson has been forgotten by Raju."}} Although both sentences indicate the same event, the meaning difference is due to the fact that in (3a) Raju, as the ''agent'', is considered to have acted volitionally (not making any special effort to retain the lesson in his memory), and is hence held responsible for that event, whereas in (3b) he, being an ''experiencer'', is involved in that event only non-volitionally, and hence one does not hold him responsible for it. ==Relating case roles to morphological case and structural Case== [[File:Relating case roles to morphological case and structural Case.png|thumb|A Venn diagram demonstrating how case role (semantic roles) relates to the morphological case, and how morphological case in turn relates to Case (structural case).]] ===Semantic vs. morphological=== The [[semantics|semantic]] category of case (specifically case role) is related to [[Grammatical case|morphological case]]. Morphological case (such as accusative, ergative, dative, genitive, and sometimes also partitive) reflects the ranking of arguments, while semantic case (such as instrumental, comitative, locative, and directional) encodes a semantic relation between the DP{{Clarify|reason=Unknown abbreviation|date=July 2024}} and the governing head.<ref name=Wunderlich>{{cite journal|last=Wunderlich|first=D|author2=R. Lakämper|title=On the interaction of structural and semantic case|year=2001|journal=Lingua|volume=111|issue=4–7|pages=377–418|doi=10.1016/s0024-3841(00)00040-1 |url=http://user.phil-fak.uni-duesseldorf.de/~wdl/Synsem.pdf}}</ref> Morphological case is typical of [[Complement (linguistics)|complements]] and is licensed by structural Case. By contrast, semantic case is typical of [[Adjunct (grammar)|adjuncts]]; it is only licensed by the meaning of the [[Head (linguistics)|head]].<ref name="Wunderlich"/> From the case roles proposed by Fillmore (1968),<ref name="Fillmore 1968"/> it was demonstrated that case roles appeared where the morphological cases of dative, genitive or instrumental appeared: *The experiencer case role (dative in many languages) *The recipient case role (dative in many languages) *The possessor case role (genitive in many languages) *The partitive case role (genitive or partitive in many languages) *The instrument case role (instrumental or dative in many languages) The following are observations from Sigurðsson<ref name="Sigurðsson">{{cite journal|last=Sigurðsson|first=H.A.|title=Case: abstract vs. morphological|journal=New Perspectives on Case Theory|year=2003|series=In E. Brandern & Zinsmeister (Ed.)|pages=223–268}}</ref> on the "case-semantics" in case-languages demonstrating how morphological case is ''not'' blind to semantics: *Agentive subjects are nominative *Indirect objects are either dative or accusative *Most benefactive (indirect or direct) objects are dative *Most malefactive (indirect or direct) objects are accusative *Instrumental DP-objects are dative *If a lexical item has a choice between an accusative or a dative complement, then that choice is normally semantically controlled ===Morphological vs. structural=== [[Grammatical case|Morphological case]] is related to structural Case (based on syntax) in the following ways: Structural Case is a condition for arguments that originates from a relational head (e.g. verb), while morphological case is a property that depends on the NP or DP complement.<ref name="Wunderlich"/> The relationship between morphological case and structural case is evident in how morphological case is subject to case agreement whereby the morphological case appearing on a DP must be licensed by the syntactic context of the DP.<ref name="Wunderlich"/> In much of the [[transformational grammar]] literature, morphological cases are viewed as determined by the syntactic configuration.<ref name=Kracht>{{cite journal|last=Kracht|first=M|title=Case at the syntax/semantics interface|journal=The Relationship Between Syntax and Semantics in the Analysis of Linguistic Structure|year=1999|series=Proceedings from the Helsinki Symposium}}</ref> The accusative case is assigned through a structural relation between the verbal head and its complement.<ref name="Kracht"/> For example, the direct [[Complement (linguistics)|complement]] of a verb is assigned accusative, irrespective of any other properties that it might have.<ref name="Kracht"/> It must be acknowledged that it is not the accusative alone that is structural, rather the [[Specifier (linguistics)|specifier]] of a NP is in the genitive in many languages, and so is the [[Object (grammar)|direct object]] of a [[Nominalization|nominalized]] verb.<ref name="Kracht"/> ==Morphological case== ===Linguistic typology of morphological case=== Case can be further divided into two categories: [[grammatical cases]] and [[thematic relation|semantic cases]]. Examples of grammatical cases are [[nominative case]], [[accusative]], [[dative]], and [[Ergative case|ergative]]. These typically code core [[grammatical relation]]s which are semantically dependent on the verb, such as [[subject (grammar)|subject]] and [[object (grammar)|object]]. Semantic (or adverbial) cases are [[instrumental case|instrumental]], [[comitative case|comitative]], and [[locative case]]s. These are semantically richer and less dependent on the verb. There exist cases, such as dative, that have both semantic and grammatical case features.<ref>{{cite book|last=Kittilä|first=S, Västi, K., & Ylikoski, J.|title=Case, animacy, and semantic roles|year=2011|publisher=John Benjamins Pub. Co|location=Philadelphia}}</ref> Eight commonly seen cases ([[Indo-european languages|Indo-European]] case): {| class="wikitable" |- ! Case !! Characteristics |- | [[Nominative case|Nominative]] || *Indicates the subject, as the specifier of a finite verb<ref name=Carnie>{{cite book|last=Carnie|first=Andrew|title=Syntax: A Generative Introduction|year=2013|publisher=Blackwell Publishing|isbn=978-0-470-65531-3|pages=335–342}}</ref> *Recent research indicates nominative is distinct from ergative case<ref name=Woolford/>{{rp|p.2}} *Some research indicates nominative is synonymous to ergative case<ref name=Woolford/>{{rp|p.4}} |- | [[Accusative case|Accusative]] || *Indicates the direct object, as the complement of a transitive verb<ref name="Carnie"/> *Lexical case associated with themes ::Ex. ''The teacher scolded '''us'''.'' |- | [[Dative case|Dative]] || *Indicates the indirect object of a verb *Lexical case associated with goals/experiences ::Ex. ''The banker handed '''us''' a pamphlet.'' ::Ex. ''The banker handed a check '''to us'''.'' |- | [[Instrumental case|Instrumental]] || *Indicates an object used in performing an action ::Ex. ''We cleaned the table '''with a wipe'''.'' ::Ex. ''Written '''by hand'''.'' |- | [[Locative case|Locative]] || *Indicates a location ::Ex. ''We live '''in Canada'''.'' |- | [[Vocative case|Vocative]] || *Indicates an addressee ::Ex. '''''Becky''', are you mad?'' ::Ex. ''Hello, '''Maria'''!'' |- | [[Genitive case|Genitive]] || *Corresponds to English’s possessive case and preposition ''of'', indicates the possessor of another noun |- | [[Ablative case|Ablative]] || *Indicates movement ''from'' something, or ''cause'' |} Precise description of the above cases vary cross-linguistically. '''Ergative case''' It has been suggested that the lexical case associated with [[agent (grammar)|agents]] is the [[Ergative case|ergative (ERG)]] lexical case: it identifies the noun as a subject of a transitive verb in languages that allow ergative case. The correlation demonstrated between the ergative case and the theta role of the agent is not a perfect correlation: It is as closely correlated as the relationship between the dative case and the theta role goals/ experiencers.<ref name=Woolford/>{{rp|p.3}} There are two types of [[Ergative–absolutive language|ergative languages]]: languages that allow ergative subjects in intransitive clauses and those that prohibit them.<ref name=Woolford/>{{rp|p.19}} There can be a distinction made between ergative and accusative type languages with respect to the type of subject and object markings they will display.<ref name=Butt>{{cite journal|last=Butt|first=Mariam|author2=King T.H.|title=The Status of Case|year=2004|pages=153–198}}</ref> This distinction is characterized by the type of clauses a language allows, such as ergative, [[Absolutive case|absolutive]], accusative, and nominative. The distinction made between language type and clause type is illustrated in the table below:<ref name=Butt/>{{rp|p.158}} {| class="wikitable" style="text-align:center;" border="1" |- ! rowspan="2"|Clause type !! colspan ="2"|Language type |- | Ergative || Accusative |- | Transitive || Erg-Abs || Nom-Acc |- | Intransitive || Abs || Nom |} '''Case assignment''' The [[Linguistic typology|typological]] differences among languages have been shown to be the consequence of differences in whether ergative, dative, or other individual case assignment is optional or obligatory in a language (although, the ergative-accusative case pattern appears to be universally blocked).<ref name=Woolford/> The class of verbs that mark their subjects with a particular lexical dative case is similar across languages, but the inclusion in this verb class is not completely predictable. Lexical cases such as nominative usually mark predictable theta roles, but there is a substantial amount of characteristic behaviour involved indicating that a verb's ability to assign a lexical case to one of its arguments must be specified in that verb's lexical entry.<ref name=Woolford/>{{rp|p.11}} Once a verb marks its subject with a given lexical case, such as nominative, that verb cannot assign the structural accusative case to its object.<ref name=Woolford/>{{rp|p.20}} [[File:Japanese case assignment.png|thumb|Example of transliterated Japanese morphologically overt case assignment. The dative particle '-ni' assigns a recipient case role to the NP 'neko', and the accusative particle '-o' assigns a theme case role to the NP 'sakana'.]] '''Morphologically overt case assignment''' In languages with case marking of explicit morphology, any [[nominal (linguistics)|nominal]] that is morphologically capable of showing case morphology is obligated to do so. This observation can be stated explicitly by what is referred to as the Case Filter.<ref name="Carnie"/> Case Filter can be further described as being an abstract Case hypothesis that stipulates all lexical noun phrases are assigned a specific Case regardless if this Case manifests at the surface level: If this lexical NP does not have a Case marking at surface structure, then the sentence that contains it is considered ungrammatical.<ref name="Carnie"/><ref name =Sobin>{{cite book|last=Sobin|first=Nicholas|title=Syntactic analysis: The basics|year=2011|publisher=Wiley-Blackwell|page=157}}</ref> Case theory includes an inventory of structural Case and a series of lexical cases that are assigned (in all languages) at the level of the [[deep structure]] in conjunction with theta role assignment.<ref name=Woolford/>{{rp|p.3}} Languages such as [[Russian language|Russian]] and [[Japanese language|Japanese]] follow a similar mechanism of case assignment as that seen in [[Latin]]. Russian is like Latin, in that it does have genitive and dative case that is assigned by the N (noun) and A (adjective or adverb). In Russian for example, most nouns show overt case morphology as does Latin, but there is also a productive class of [[Latin declension|indeclinable nouns]].<ref name=Torrego/>{{rp|p.3}} These indeclinable nouns are not able to receive case morphology.<ref name=Torrego/>{{rp|p.3}} Examples from languages exhibiting morphologically overt case marking indicate that there are rules of case assignment present in the [[grammar]] of a language. To account for this, rules can be generated as support. For example, support accounting for accusative cases in Latin-type case-marked languages could be presented as:<ref name=Torrego/> {| class="wikitable" |- | (i) || V (verb) and P (preposition) assign accusative case to an NP complement Accusative case is assigned to the complements of V and in some instances P |- | (ii) || N (noun) and A (adverb/adjective) do not assign accusative case (to an NP complement) Accusative case is not found on the complement of either N or A |} Application of these accusative case assignment rules can be seen in the following examples:<ref name=Torrego/> {{interlinear|number=(4a) |top= Complement to V (accusative) | VP scripsit libr-um | {} wrote book-ACC |}} {{interlinear|number=(4b) |top= Complement to P (accusative) | PP ad Hispani-am | {} to Spain-ACC |}} For the morphologically rich case language of Latin, there are seven cases for Latin nouns. The following table demonstrates Latin case morphology assignment for a masculine noun ''somnus'', meaning 'sleep'.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Mathews|first=Peter H.|title=Inflectional morphology: A theoretical study based on aspects of Latin verb conjugation|journal=CUP Archive|year=1972|volume=6}}</ref> {| class="wikitable" |- ! Case !! Singular form !! Plural form |- | [[Nominative case|Nominative]] :''nominativus'' || somnus || somni |- | [[Accusative case|Accusative]] :''accusativus'' ||somnum ||somnos |- | [[Genitive case|Genitive]] :''genitivus'' ||somni ||somnorum |- | [[Dative case|Dative]] :''dativus'' ||somno ||somnis |- | [[Ablative case|Ablative]] :''ablativus'' ||somno ||somnis |- | [[Locative case|Locative]] :''locativus'' ||somni ||somnis |- | [[Vocative case|Vocative]] :''vocativus'' ||somne ||somni |} '''Modern German''' In modern [[German language|German]], all noun phrases show case forms with case generally being marked on the determiner. However, for the genitive form German marks the noun with a morphologically overt case form (this case form is retained on noun phrases in modern English, surfacing as -'s).<ref name=Sobin/>{{rp|p.148}} The following table illustrates the masculine case forms of German.<ref name=Sobin/>{{rp|p.148}} {| class="wikitable" |- ! Case !! "the man" !! "he" |- | [[Nominative case|Nominative]] ||der Mann ||er (he) |- | [[Accusative case|Accusative]] ||den Mann ||ihn (him) |- | [[Dative case|Dative]] ||dem Mann ||ihm ((to) him) |- | [[Genitive case|Genitive]] ||des Mannes ||sein (his) |- |} ===Case hierarchy=== {{main article|Case hierarchy}} [[File:Accusative alignment.svg|thumbnail|Schematic representation of nominative-accusative alignment. Subject of intransitive verb (S) and subject of transitive verb (A) are treated similarly while object of transitive verb (O or P) is treated differently.]][[File:Ergative alignment.svg|thumbnail|Schematic representation of ergative-absolutive alignment. Subject of intransitive verb (S) and object of transitive verb (O or P) are treated similarly while subject of transitive verb (A) is treated differently.]] As discussed in [[Case role#Linguistic typology of morphological case|linguistic typology of morphological case]], there can be a number of cases found within a single language. In a sample of case systems, they appear in a particular order and a hierarchy emerges. This specific ordering is called the [[case hierarchy]]. [[Nominative case|Nominative]]/[[Absolutive case|Absolutive]] → [[Accusative case|Accusative]]/[[Ergative case|Ergative]] → [[Genitive case|Genitive]] → [[Dative case|Dative]] → [[Ablative case|Ablative]]/[[Instrumental case|Instrumental]] → Other [[Oblique case|oblique]] arguments<ref name=Blake>{{cite journal|last=Blake|first=Barry J.|title=The case hierarchy|journal=La Trobe Working Papers in Linguistics|year=1992|volume=5}}</ref> This hierarchy is to be interpreted as follows: If a language has a case that is listed on the hierarchy, it will usually have at least one case from each position to the left. If a language has a dative case, it will have a genitive (position to the left), an accusative or ergative case or both, and a nominative.<ref name=Blake/> While the case hierarchy holds for many languages, it is not universally valid. Some languages may lack one of the above categories or may collapse categories into one hierarchy position.<ref>{{cite book|last=Jensen|first=J.T|title=Case and thematic role in Latin: Evidence from passive constructions|publisher=Indiana University Linguistics Club|year=1983}}</ref> There are times when none of the cases in one system will correspond with any of the cases in the other system. For example, the dative in Ancient Greek does not correspond closely with the dative of Latin. So when comparing cases across languages, the functions of a particular case need to be considered, rather than the labels the language has assigned.<ref name=Blake/> ==== Examples of two case and three case systems within the case hierarchy ==== The hierarchy predicts that it is possible for a language to only have two cases. There are a number of languages that have a two-case system, such as [[Chemehuevi#Language|Chemeheuvi]], [[Kabardian language|Kabardian]], as well as in [[Iranian languages]]. Chemeheuvi is a language of the [[Numic languages|Numic]] branch of [[Uto-Aztecan languages|Uto-Aztecan]] family which has a nominative-oblique system. [[Yaghnobi language|Yagnobi]], an Iranian language, has both an accusative and ergative case.<ref name="Blake" /> [[Semitic languages]], [[Nubian languages]], [[Modern Greek]] operate with a three-case systems, with a nominative, accusative, and genitive/oblique case.<ref name=Blake/> ==Structural Case== === Examples of Structural Case in English === Morphological Case is the surface Case form which is assigned to either a noun phrase or a pronoun phrase depending on its surface position within a sentence.<ref name=Sobin/>{{rp|p.157}} [[English language|English]] case morphology is very sparse or in some instances, non-existent. The distribution of nominals in languages such as English, where there is a lack of case morphology, is governed by the same precise laws that also regulate nominative and accusative case in morphologically overt case-marked languages such as Latin and Russian.<ref name=Torrego>{{cite book|last=Torrego|first=Esther|title=The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Minimalism|page=13}}</ref> For example the distribution of accusative case: :Accusative case assignment:<ref name=Torrego/>{{rp|p.4}} ::α assigns accusative case to β only if: :::iii. α is V or P (not N or A); and :::iv. β is the complement of α In English, the object of a noun phrase is assigned a Case by the closest [[c-command]]ing V (verb) or P (preposition) head, which is usually the verb or preposition that selects it as a complement. Similarly, the subject of a noun phrase is assigned Case from the finite [[Grammatical tense|tense]] head, T. The subject of a [[Non-finite clause|finite clause]] in English is nominative. As seen in English with sentences in passive form, the finite T is the head that assigns the nominative Case to the noun phrase (subject position); the closest noun phrase that T (tense head) c-commands is attracted to the empty specifier position of the [[Grammatical tense|tense phrase]].<ref name=Sobin/>{{rp|p.149}} This movement is a local specifier-to-specifier movement, satisfying Case requirements at the deep-structure level. Any sentence in English that surfaces with a noun phrase not containing Case, is a violation of the Case Filter and thus is said to 'crash' (determined to be ungrammatical).<ref name=Sobin/>{{rp|p.149}} {| class="wikitable" |- ! !! NP complements in English<ref name=Torrego/>{{rp|p.2}} !! Examples<ref name=Torrego/>{{rp|p.2}} |- |(5a) |V and P allow an NP complement. ||[VP wrote the book] [PP to Spain] |- |(5b) |N and A do not allow an NP complement. ||*[NP love liberty] [*AP free luxuries] |- |} Although English does not have [[Null morpheme|non-zero case morphology]], except with respect to [[personal pronoun]]s, it satisfies the Case Filter. The phenomenon of phonologically zero case morphology is found in languages where the presence of a rich case system is not in doubt. English has an abstract or non-overt variant of case. For example, accusative case may be assigned to a nominal complement of V (verb) or P (preposition) by either V (verb) or P (preposition), but these complements receiving case show no overt morphology reflecting this process.<ref name=Torrego/>{{rp|p.3}} English lacks formal cases such as [[vocative case]] and [[ablative case]] (which can be assigned to nominals); complements to the heads assigning case will not receive an abstract marking of case, and will be excluded by the case filter.<ref name=Torrego/>{{rp|p.3}} {| class="wikitable" |- ! !! Differences between English and Latin<ref name=Torrego/>{{rp|p.2}} !! Similarities between English and Latin<ref name=Torrego/>{{rp|p.2}} |- | (6a) || Case morphology in English is phonologically zero (excluding personal pronouns) || English has accusative case, but not dative and ablative like Latin |- | (6b) || English allows nominal complements in the same contexts Latin assigns accusative case || English does not allow bare nominal complements in the same locations that Latin disallows accusative case |} Pronouns in English, however, change forms when they change case. These changes are clearly seen with [[personal pronoun]]s for example: [[Grammatical person|first person]], [[Grammatical person|second person]], and [[Grammatical person|third person]] are represented as 'I', 'you', and 'he' or 'she', respectively. Subjects of active voice sentences typically in English take on the nominative Case and objects the accusative Case.<ref name=Sobin/>{{rp|p.148}} Personal pronouns in English can be classified into three Case categories: {| class="wikitable" |- ! Case !! Used for !! Example |- | [[Nominative case|Nominative]]|| *NOM marks the subject of a [[finite verb]] *Sometimes for the [[Complement (linguistics)|complement]] of a [[Copula (linguistics)|copula]] || [[Subjective pronouns]]: ''I, he, she, we, they'' Example: "'''They''' want an A" |- | [[Accusative case|Accusative]] || *The direct or indirect [[Object (grammar)|object]] of a verb *The object of a preposition (in some languages) *Sometimes for the complement of a copula *ACC is marked on the noun that is having something done to it *Syntactic function: designates the immediate object of an action || [[Object pronoun]]s: ''me, him, her, us, them'' Example: "The teacher scolded '''us'''" |- | [[Genitive case|Genitive]] || *GEN marks the grammatical possessor *GEN marking for the possessor can be seen as -'s and as possessive pronouns || [[Possessive pronouns]]: ''my/mine, his, her(s), our(s), your(s), their(s)''<ref name=Sobin/>{{rp|p.148}} Example: "That is '''our''' pen" |} English has case forms for pronouns for genitive, accusative, and nominative cases. Conversely, English has only genitive case forms for 'full [[noun phrase]]s' (or determiner phrases): For example, "John''''s''' blue cat" is assigned genitive case.<ref name=Sobin/>{{rp|p.148}} Full noun phrases cannot be assigned accusative or nominative case. The possessive marking ''-'s'' as a case ending attaches to the end of a noun phrase, not necessarily to the head noun itself. Example: English Noun Phrase Marked with Genitive Case<ref name=Sobin/>{{rp|p.148}} : [<sub>NP</sub> [<sub>NP</sub> The boy] '''-'s'''<sub>poss</sub> ball] It is a question of present-day research, why is it that with pronouns in English, Case marking is preserved? A proposed answer has been that since pronouns are a closed category and don't partake in productive morphology, then they are in a way "memorized" by speakers of English.<ref name=Sobin/>{{rp|p.149}} The fact that they may be just memorized forms suggests that there is no significant reason for why members of this category should undergo a drastic change, losing morphologically overt Case marking. Considering this argument, why then do noun phrases receive genitive Case marking in English? The notion of abstract Case marking on all noun phrases can help answer this question: All noun phrases undergo Case marking assignment but this is not always phonetically realized at the surface structure (the exception to surfacing of this morphological case is the possessive form '''-'s''').<ref name=Sobin/>{{rp|p.149}} There is a phenomenon in English known as [[exceptional case-marking]] (ECM),<ref name=Torrego/>{{rp|p.10}} which makes it evident that accusative case is not necessarily assigned to the complement of the assigner. The following example is intended to demonstrate that case can be assigned as long as there is no barrier that intervenes, and given that there is no nominal closer in proximity to the assigner than the assignee.<ref name=Torrego/>{{rp|p.10}} For example (* indicates an ungrammaticality):<ref name=Torrego/>{{rp|p.10}} {| class="wikitable" |- ! !! Examples |- | (7a) || ''Mary believes [Sue to have read the book].'' |- | (7b) || ''*Mary's belief [Sue to have read the book].'' |- | (7c) || ''Sue was believed [_ to have read the book].'' |} ==Debate about case role== There have been arguments made for case role to be considered a universal in language, for several reasons. In previous studies, primarily during the early 20th century, where case role in languages was considered using [[Latin]] or [[Greek language|Greek]] classification, it was found that there were difficulties in approaching other languages such as [[Aleut]] or [[Thai language|Thai]].<ref name=Fillmore/> These methods involved illustrating semantic relationships of given case forms and did not come from a primarily syntactical standpoint. Case role in Latin and Greek classifications tended to neglect nominative case as well, and instead focused on creating different semantic classes for other cases, such as [[Dative#Latin|the dative of possession]].<ref name=Fillmore/> Because there have been several such problems analyzing case role cross-linguistically when using one language as a standard, it is not common practice to take traditional Latin or Greek classifications. Instead, the particular languages' syntactic structure forms the base for analyzing semantic value and case role in that language.<ref name=Fillmore/> There are still questions regarding case morphology. One approach to defining case morphology is to say that it is the presence of some special morphology, the shape of which has a correlation with a specific syntactic position.<ref name=Torrego/>{{rp|p.1}} Decades of past research support this being a particular cross-linguistic property specific to [[nominal (linguistics)|nominal]]s. When discussing case features, it is commonly stated that they have no associated semantic interpretation, regardless of their syntactic position.<ref name=Torrego/>{{rp|p.14}} An early perspective taken on case was the initial suggestion by French linguist [[:fr:Jean-Roger Vergnaud|Jean-Roger Vergnaud]] that the grammar of case is a cross-linguistic feature, central to the syntax of all languages and that it does not just apply to languages with a rich case morphology, such as Latin. Vergnaud stipulated that any nominal that is morphologically able to show case morphology is required and must do so. This observation was explicitly stated above as being the definition of Case Filter.<ref name=Torrego/>{{rp|p.3}} A second perspective on Case is [[Minimalist program|Chomsky's Minimalist conjectures]]. There are many fundamental questions regarding case role, such as the reason why case should even exist, that are not yet accompanied by substantive answers. This indicates that research on case role remains still as a "work in progress".<ref name=Torrego/>{{rp|p.19}} ==See also== * [[Case grammar]] * [[Case hierarchy]] * [[Grammatical case]] * [[Thematic relations]] * [[Theta roles]] ==References== {{reflist}} [[Category:Grammatical cases]]
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Pages transcluded onto the current version of this page
(
help
)
:
Template:Cite book
(
edit
)
Template:Cite journal
(
edit
)
Template:Clarification needed
(
edit
)
Template:Clarify
(
edit
)
Template:Interlinear
(
edit
)
Template:Main article
(
edit
)
Template:Reflist
(
edit
)
Template:Rp
(
edit
)