Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Cognitivism (psychology)
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
{{short description|Theoretical framework for understanding the mind}} {{Other uses|Cognitivism (disambiguation)}} {{Psychology sidebar|basic}} {{Circular|date=November 2023}} In [[psychology]], '''cognitivism''' is a theoretical framework for understanding the mind that gained credence in the 1950s. The movement was a response to [[behaviorism]], which cognitivists said neglected to explain [[cognition]]. [[Cognitive psychology]] derived its name from the Latin ''cognoscere'', referring to knowing and information, thus cognitive psychology is an information-processing psychology derived in part from earlier traditions of the investigation of thought and problem solving.<ref>Mandler, G. (2002). Origins of the cognitive (r)evolution. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 38, 339–353</ref><ref name="Lilien-1">{{Citation |last=Lilienfeld |first=S. |title=Psychology: A Framework for Everyday Thinking |pages=24–28 |year=2010 |publisher=Pearson |isbn=978-0-205-65048-4 |last2=Lynn |first2=S. J. |last3=Namy |first3=L. |last4=Woolf |first4=N.}}</ref> Behaviorists acknowledged the existence of thinking but identified it as a behavior. Cognitivists argued that the way people think impacts their behavior and therefore cannot be a behavior in and of itself. Cognitivists later claimed that thinking is so essential to psychology that the study of thinking should become its own field.<ref name="Lilien-1" /> However, cognitivists typically presuppose a specific form of mental activity, of the kind advanced by [[computationalism]]. Cognitivism has more recently been challenged by [[postcognitivism]]. == Cognitive development == The process of assimilating and expanding our intellectual horizon is termed as [[cognitive]] development. We have a complex [[physiological]] structure that absorbs a variety of [[Stimulus (physiology)|stimuli]] from the [[environment (systems)|environment]], stimuli being the interactions that are able to produce [[knowledge]] and skills. Parents process knowledge informally in the home while teachers process knowledge formally in school. [[Knowledge]] should be pursued with zest and zeal; if not, then learning becomes a burden.<ref>{{Cite book |title=General Psychology |publisher=Neelkamal |edition=First edition, 2004 |page=60}}</ref> == Attention == [[Attention]] is the first part of cognitive development. It pertains to a person's ability to focus and sustain concentration.<ref name=":0">{{Cite book |last=Siegel |first=L. S. |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=C1JDBAAAQBAJ&q=attention+cognitive+development&pg=PT175 |title=Cognitive Development in Atypical Children: Progress in Cognitive Development Research |last2=Morrison |first2=F. J. |date=2012-12-06 |publisher=Springer Science & Business Media |isbn=9781461250364 |language=en}}</ref> Attention can also be how focus minded an individual is and having their full concentration on one thing.<ref>{{Citation |last=Friedrich |first=Frances |title=7.2 Attention |date=2019-06-28 |work=Introduction to Psychology |url=https://openpress.usask.ca/introductiontopsychology/chapter/attention/ |access-date=2021-03-18 |publisher=University of Saskatchewan Open Press |language=en}}</ref> It is differentiated from other [[Temperament|temperamental characteristics]] like persistence and distractibility in the sense that the latter modulates an individual's daily interaction with the environment.<ref name=":0" /> Attention, on the other hand, involves his behavior when performing specific tasks.<ref name=":0" /> Learning, for instance, takes place when the student gives attention towards the teacher. Interest and effort closely relate to attention. Attention is an active process which involves numerous outside stimuli. The attention of an organism at any point in time involves three concentric circles; beyond awareness, margin, and focus.<ref>{{Cite book |title=General Psychology |publisher=Neelkamal |edition=First edition, 2004 |page=59}}</ref> Individuals have a mental capacity; there are only so many things someone can focus on at one time. A theory of cognitive development called information processing holds that memory and attention are the foundation of cognition. It is suggested that children's attention is initially selective and is based on situations that are important to their goals.<ref name=":1">{{Cite book |last=Pendergast |first=Donna |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=0QxvDwAAQBAJ&q=attention+cognitive+development+learning&pg=PT44 |title=Teaching Primary Years: Rethinking curriculum, pedagogy and assessment |last2=Main |first2=Katherine |date=2019-01-07 |publisher=Allen & Unwin |isbn=9781760870362 |language=en}}</ref> This capacity increases as the child grows older since they are more able to absorb stimuli from tasks.<ref name=":1" /> Another conceptualization classified attention into mental attention and perceptual attention. The former is described as the executive-driven attentional "brain energy" that activates task-relevant processes in the brain while the latter are immediate or spontaneous attention driven by novel perceptual experiences.<ref>{{Cite book |last=Barrouillet |first=Pierre |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=F-J4AgAAQBAJ&q=attention+cognitive+development&pg=PT40 |title=Cognitive Development and Working Memory: A Dialogue between Neo-Piagetian Theories and Cognitive Approaches |last2=Gaillard |first2=Vinciane |date=2010-12-21 |publisher=Psychology Press |isbn=9781136930058 |language=en}}</ref> == Process of learning == [[Cognitive]] theory mainly stresses the acquisition of [[knowledge]] and growth of the mental structure. [[Cognitive]] theory tends to focus on conceptualizing the student's [[learning]] process: how information is received; how information is processed and organized into existing schema; how information is retrieved upon recall. In other words, cognitive theory seeks to explain the process of [[knowledge]] acquisition and the subsequent effects on the mental structures within the mind. Learning is not about the mechanics of what a learner does, but rather a process depending on what the learner already knows (existing information) and their method of acquiring new [[knowledge]] (how they integrate new information into their existing schemas).<ref>{{Cite web |last=Kurt |first=Dr Serhat |date=2023-10-25 |title=Cognitivism Learning Theory, Strategies and Examples |url=https://educationaltechnology.net/cognitivism-learning-theory-strategies-and-examples/ |access-date=2023-10-25 |website=Educational Technology |language=en-US}}</ref> Knowledge acquisition is an activity consisting of internal codification of mental structures within the student's mind. Inherent to the theory, the student must be an active participant in their own learning process. Cognitive approaches mainly focus on the mental activities of the learner like mental planning, goal setting, and organizational strategies.<ref>Shell, 1980</ref> In [[cognitive]] theories not only the environmental factors and instructional components play an important role in learning. There are additional key elements like learning to code, transform, rehearse, and store and retrieve the information. The learning process includes learner's thoughts, beliefs, and attitude values.<ref>Winna, 1988</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last=Ertmer |first=Peggy A. |last2=Newby |first2=Timothy J. |year=1993 |title=Behaviorism, Cognitivism, Constructivism: Comparing Critical Features from an Instructional Design Perspective |url=http://northweststate.edu/wp-content/uploads/files/21143_ftp.pdf |url-status=dead |journal=Performance Improvement Quarterly |publisher=Wiley |volume=6 |issue=4 |pages=50–72 |doi=10.1111/j.1937-8327.1993.tb00605.x |issn=0898-5952 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20150413055145/northweststate.edu/wp-content/uploads/files/21143_ftp.pdf |archive-date=2015-04-13}}</ref> == Role of memory == [[Memory]] plays a vital role in the [[learning]] process. Information is stored within memory in an organised, meaningful manner. Here, teacher and designers play different roles in the [[learning]] process. Teachers supposedly facilitate learning and the organization of information in an optimal way. Whereas designers supposedly use advanced techniques (such as analogies, mnemonic devices, and hierarchical relationships) to help learners acquire new information to add to their prior [[knowledge]]. Forgetting is described as an inability to retrieve information from memory. [[Memory]] loss may be a mechanism used to discard situationally irrelevant information by assessing the relevance of newly acquired information.<ref>{{Cite web |title=Teaching Cognitivism |url=http://teachinglearningresources.pbworks.com/w/page/31012664/Cognitivism}}</ref> == Process of transfer == According to [[cognitive]] theory, if a learner knows how to implement [[knowledge]] in different contexts and conditions, then we can say that transfer has occurred.<ref name="Schunk, 1991">Schunk, 1991</ref> Understanding is composed of knowledge - in the form of rules, concepts and discrimination.<ref>Duffy and Jonassen, 1991</ref> Knowledge stored in [[memory]] is important, but the use of such knowledge is also important. Prior knowledge will be used for identifying similarities and differences between itself and novel [[information]].<ref>{{Cite web |title=Cognitivism |url=http://www.learning-theories.com/cognitivism.html |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20220804130251/https://learning-theories.com/cognitivism.html |archive-date=2022-08-04 |website=Learning Theories}}</ref> == Types of learning explained in detail by this position == Cognitive theory mostly explains complex forms of learning in terms of reasoning, problem solving and information processing.<ref name="Schunk, 1991" /> Emphasis must be placed on the fact that the goal of all aforementioned viewpoints is considered to be the same - the transfer of knowledge to the student in the most efficient and effective manner possible.<ref>Bednar et al., 1991<!-- Pretty sure this is Bednar, Cunningham, Duffy, & Perry, 1991, which is chapter 2 of a book, but couldn't find the name of that book. --></ref> Simplification and standardization are two techniques used to enhance the effectiveness and efficiency of knowledge transfer. Knowledge can be analysed, decomposed and simplified into basic building blocks. There is a correlation with the behaviorist model of the knowledge transfer environment. Cognitivists stress the importance of efficient processing strategies.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Ertmer |first=Peggy A. |last2=Newby |first2=Timothy J. |date=2013 |title=Behaviorism, Cognitivism, Constructivism: Comparing Critical Features From an Instructional Design Perspective |url=https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/piq.21143 |journal=Performance Improvement Quarterly |language=en |volume=26 |issue=2 |pages=43–71 |doi=10.1002/piq.21143|url-access=subscription }}</ref> == Basic principles of the cognitive theory and relevance to instructional design == A behaviorist uses feedback (reinforcement) to change the behavior in the desired direction, while the cognitivist uses the feedback for guiding and supporting the accurate [[mind|mental]] connections.<ref name="Thompson et al., 1992">Thompson, A., Simonson, M., & Hargrave, C. (1992). Educational technology: A review of the research. Washington, DC: Association for Educational Communications and Technology.</ref> For different reasons learners' task analyzers are critical to both cognitivists and behaviorists. Cognitivists look at the learner's predisposition to learning (How does the learner activate, maintain, and direct their learning?).<ref name="Thompson et al., 1992" /> Additionally, cognitivists examine the learners' 'how to design' [[Teaching|instruction]] that it can be assimilated. (i.e., what about the learner's existing mental structures?) In contrast, the behaviorists look to determine where the lesson should begin (i.e., at what level the learners are performing successfully?) and what are the most effective reinforcements (i.e., What are the consequences that are most desired by the learner?). There are some specific assumptions or principles that direct the instructional design: active involvement of the learner in the learning process, learner control, metacognitive training (e.g., self-planning, monitoring, and revising techniques), the use of hierarchical analyses to identify and illustrate prerequisite relationships (cognitive task analysis procedure), facilitating optimal processing of structuring, organizing and sequencing information (use of cognitive strategies such as outlining, summaries, synthesizers, advance organizers etc.), encouraging the students to make connections with previously learned material, and creating [[learning]] environments (recall of prerequisite skills; use of relevant examples, analogies). == Structuring instruction == [[Cognitive]] theories emphasize mainly on making [[knowledge]] meaningful and helping learners to organize and relate new [[information]] to existing [[knowledge]] in [[memory]]. Instruction should be based on students' existing schema or mental structures, to be effective. The organisation of information is connected in such a manner that it should relate to the existing knowledge in some meaningful way. Examples of [[cognitive]] strategies include the use of analogies and metaphors, framing, outlining the mnemonics, concept mapping, advance organizers, and so forth.<ref>West, Charles K., James A. Farmer, and Phillip M. Wolff. Instructional design: Implications from cognitive science. Prentice Hall, 1991.</ref> The cognitive theory mainly emphasizes the major tasks of the teacher / designer and includes analyzing various learning experiences to the learning situation, which can impact learning outcomes of different individuals. Organizing and structuring the new information to connect the learners' previously acquired knowledge abilities and experiences. The new information is effectively and efficiently assimilated/accommodated within the learners cognitive structure.<ref>Stepich and Newby, 1988<!-- this is either https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00052699 or https://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pfi.4170270911 --></ref> ==Theoretical approach== Cognitivism has two major components, one methodological, the other theoretical. Methodologically, cognitivism has a [[positivist]] approach and says that psychology can be (in principle) fully explained by the use of the [[scientific method]], there is speculation on whether or not this is true.<ref name="psychegames.com">{{Cite web |title=Cognitivism |url=https://www.psychegames.com/cognitivism.htm |access-date=2020-01-29 |website=Psyche Games}}</ref>{{Circular reference|date=November 2023}} This is also largely a [[reductionist]] goal, with the belief that individual components of mental function (the 'cognitive architecture') can be identified and meaningfully understood.<ref>{{Cite web |title=Components of Cognitive Theory: Learning Sciences Resource Guide |url=https://canvas.vt.edu/courses/62492/pages/components-of-cognitive-theory |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20200718144337/https://canvas.vt.edu/courses/62492/pages/components-of-cognitive-theory |archive-date=2020-07-18 |access-date=2020-05-15 |website=canvas.vt.edu}}</ref> The second says that [[cognition]] contains discrete and internal mental states ([[representations]] or [[symbols]]) that can be changed using rules or [[algorithm]]s.<ref name="psychegames.com" />{{Circular reference|date=November 2023}} Cognitivism became the dominant force in [[psychology]] in the late-20th century, replacing [[behaviorism]] as the most popular paradigm for understanding mental function. [[Cognitive psychology]] is not a wholesale refutation of behaviorism, but rather an expansion that accepts that mental states exist. This was due to the increasing criticism towards the end of the 1950s of simplistic learning models. One of the most notable criticisms was [[Noam Chomsky]]'s argument that language could not be acquired purely through conditioning, and must be at least partly explained by the existence of internal mental states. The main issues that interest cognitive psychologists are the inner mechanisms of human thought and the processes of knowing. Cognitive psychologists have attempted to shed some light on the alleged mental structures that stand in a causal relationship to our physical actions. ==Criticisms of psychological cognitivism== {{more citations needed|section|date=December 2020}} In the 1990s, various new theories emerged that challenged cognitivism and the idea that thought was best described as computation. Some of these new approaches, often influenced by phenomenological and [[postmodern philosophy]], include [[situated cognition]], [[distributed cognition]], [[dynamicism]] and [[embodied cognition]]. Some thinkers working in the field of [[artificial life]] (for example [[Rodney Brooks]]) have also produced non-cognitivist models of cognition. On the other hand, much of early cognitive psychology, and the work of many currently active cognitive psychologists, does not treat cognitive processes as computational. The idea that mental functions can be described as information processing models has been criticised by [[philosopher]] [[John Searle]] and [[mathematician]] [[Roger Penrose]] who both argue that computation has some inherent shortcomings which cannot capture the fundamentals of mental processes. * Penrose uses [[Gödel's incompleteness theorem]] (which states that there are mathematical truths which can never be proven in a sufficiently strong mathematical system; any sufficiently strong system of axioms will also be incomplete) and [[Alan Turing|Turing's]] [[halting problem]] (which states that there are some things which are inherently non-computable) as evidence for his position. * Searle has developed two arguments, the first (well known through his [[Chinese room]] [[thought experiment]]) is the '[[syntax]] is not [[semantics]]' argument—that a program is just syntax, while understanding requires semantics; therefore programs (hence cognitivism) cannot explain understanding. Such an argument presupposes the controversial notion of a [[private language]]. The second, which Searle now prefers but is less well known, is his 'syntax is not physics' argument—nothing in the world is intrinsically a computer program except as applied, described, or interpreted by an observer, so either everything can be described as a computer and trivially a brain can but then this does not explain any specific mental processes, or there is nothing intrinsic in a brain that makes it a computer (program). Many oppose these views and have criticized his arguments, which have created significant disagreement.<ref>{{Citation |title=Encyclopedia of Neuroscience |date=2009 |page=697 |chapter=Chinese Room Argument |doi=10.1007/978-3-540-29678-2_990 |isbn=978-3-540-23735-8}}</ref> Both points, Searle claims, refute cognitivism. Another argument against cognitivism is the problems of [[Ryle's Regress]] or the [[homunculus fallacy]]. Cognitivists have offered a number of arguments attempting to refute these attacks.{{Clarify|date=April 2013}} ==See also== {{div col}} * {{annotated link|Cognition}} * {{annotated link|Cognitive psychology}} * {{annotated link|Cognitive revolution}} * {{annotated link|Cognitive science}} * {{annotated link|Computational theory of mind}} * {{annotated link|Consciousness}} * {{annotated link|Critical psychology}} * {{annotated link|Educational psychology}} * {{annotated link|Enactivism}} * {{annotated link|Mentalism (psychology)}} * {{annotated link|Phenomenology (philosophy)|Phenomenology}} * {{annotated link|Postcognitivism}} * {{annotated link|Symbol grounding problem}} {{div col end}} ==References== {{Reflist}} ==Further reading== * Costall, A. and Still, A. (eds) (1987) ''Cognitive Psychology in Question''. Brighton: Harvester Press Ltd. {{ISBN|0-7108-1057-1}} * Searle, J. R. [https://web.archive.org/web/20020405095755/http://cogsci.soton.ac.uk/~harnad/Papers/Py104/searle.comp.html ''Is the brain a digital computer''] APA Presidential Address * Wallace, B ., Ross, A., Davies, J.B., and Anderson T., (eds) (2007) ''The Mind, the Body and the World: Psychology after Cognitivism''. London: Imprint Academic. {{ISBN|978-1-84540-073-6}} {{Psychology|state=collapsed}} {{Evolutionary psychology}} {{DEFAULTSORT:Cognitivism (Psychology)}} [[Category:Cognitive psychology]] [[Category:Cognitive science]] [[Category:Philosophy of psychology]] [[Category:Psychological concepts]] [[Category:Psychological schools]] [[Category:Psychological theories]]
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Pages transcluded onto the current version of this page
(
help
)
:
Template:Annotated link
(
edit
)
Template:Circular
(
edit
)
Template:Circular reference
(
edit
)
Template:Citation
(
edit
)
Template:Cite book
(
edit
)
Template:Cite journal
(
edit
)
Template:Cite web
(
edit
)
Template:Clarify
(
edit
)
Template:Div col
(
edit
)
Template:Div col end
(
edit
)
Template:Evolutionary psychology
(
edit
)
Template:ISBN
(
edit
)
Template:More citations needed
(
edit
)
Template:Other uses
(
edit
)
Template:Psychology
(
edit
)
Template:Psychology sidebar
(
edit
)
Template:Reflist
(
edit
)
Template:Short description
(
edit
)
Template:Sidebar with collapsible lists
(
edit
)