Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Computational neuroscience
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
{{short description|Branch of neuroscience}} '''Computational neuroscience''' (also known as '''theoretical neuroscience''' or '''mathematical neuroscience''') is a branch of [[neuroscience]] which employs [[mathematics]], [[computer science]], theoretical analysis and abstractions of the brain to understand the principles that govern the [[Developmental neuroscience|development]], [[Neuroanatomy|structure]], [[Neurophysiology|physiology]] and [[Cognitive neuroscience|cognitive abilities]] of the [[nervous system]].<ref name=":0">{{Cite book|title=Fundamentals of Computational Neuroscience|url=https://archive.org/details/fundamentalscomp00ttra|url-access=limited|last=Trappenberg|first=Thomas P.|publisher=Oxford University Press Inc.|year=2010|isbn=978-0-19-851582-1|location=United States|pages=[https://archive.org/details/fundamentalscomp00ttra/page/n17 2]}}</ref><ref>{{cite book |chapter=What is computational neuroscience? |author1=Patricia S. Churchland |author2=Christof Koch |author3=Terrence J. Sejnowski |title=Computational Neuroscience |pages=46–55 |editor1=Eric L. Schwartz |year=1993 |publisher=MIT Press |url=http://mitpress.mit.edu/catalog/item/default.asp?ttype=2&tid=7195 |access-date=2009-06-11 |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110604124206/http://mitpress.mit.edu/catalog/item/default.asp?ttype=2&tid=7195 |archive-date=2011-06-04 }}</ref><ref>{{cite book |author1=Dayan P. |author-link=Peter Dayan |author2=Abbott, L. F. |author2-link=Larry Abbott |title=Theoretical neuroscience: computational and mathematical modeling of neural systems |publisher=MIT Press |location=Cambridge, Mass |year=2001 |isbn=978-0-262-04199-7 }}</ref><ref>{{ cite book | author1= Gerstner, W. | author2 = Kistler, W. | author3 = Naud, R. | author4 = Paninski, L.| title = Neuronal Dynamics | publisher = Cambridge University Press | location = Cambridge, UK | year = 2014 | isbn = 9781107447615}}</ref> Computational neuroscience employs computational simulations<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Fan |first1=Xue |last2=Markram |first2=Henry |date=2019 |title=A Brief History of Simulation Neuroscience |journal=Frontiers in Neuroinformatics |volume=13 |page=32 |doi=10.3389/fninf.2019.00032 |doi-access=free |issn=1662-5196 |pmc=6513977 |pmid=31133838}}</ref> to validate and solve mathematical models, and so can be seen as a sub-field of theoretical neuroscience; however, the two fields are often synonymous.<ref>{{Cite book|last=Thomas|first=Trappenberg|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=4PDsA1EVCx0C|title=Fundamentals of Computational Neuroscience|publisher=OUP Oxford|year=2010|isbn=978-0199568413|location=|pages=2|access-date=17 January 2017}}</ref> The term mathematical neuroscience is also used sometimes, to stress the quantitative nature of the field.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Gutkin|first1=Boris|last2=Pinto|first2=David|last3=Ermentrout|first3=Bard|date=2003-03-01|title=Mathematical neuroscience: from neurons to circuits to systems|journal=Journal of Physiology-Paris|series=Neurogeometry and visual perception|volume=97|issue=2|pages=209–219|doi=10.1016/j.jphysparis.2003.09.005|pmid=14766142|s2cid=10040483|issn=0928-4257}}</ref> Computational neuroscience focuses on the description of [[Biology|biologically]] plausible [[neuron]]s (and [[Nervous system|neural systems]]) and their physiology and dynamics, and it is therefore not directly concerned with biologically unrealistic models used in [[connectionism]], [[control theory]], [[cybernetics]], [[quantitative psychology]], [[machine learning]], [[artificial neural network]]s, [[artificial intelligence]] and [[computational learning theory]];<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Kriegeskorte|first1=Nikolaus|last2=Douglas|first2=Pamela K.|date=September 2018|title=Cognitive computational neuroscience|journal=Nature Neuroscience|language=en|volume=21|issue=9|pages=1148–1160|doi=10.1038/s41593-018-0210-5|pmid=30127428|pmc=6706072|arxiv=1807.11819|bibcode=2018arXiv180711819K|issn=1546-1726}}</ref><ref>{{Citation |last=Paolo |first=E. D. |title=Organismically-inspired robotics: homeostatic adaptation and teleology beyond the closed sensorimotor loop | journal=Dynamical Systems Approach to Embodiment and Sociality |s2cid=15349751 }}</ref> <ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Brooks|first1=R.|last2=Hassabis|first2=D.|last3=Bray|first3=D.|last4=Shashua|first4=A.|date=2012-02-22|title=Turing centenary: Is the brain a good model for machine intelligence?|journal=Nature|language=En|volume=482|issue=7386|pages=462–463|bibcode=2012Natur.482..462.|doi=10.1038/482462a|issn=0028-0836|pmid=22358812|s2cid=205070106|doi-access=free}}</ref> although mutual inspiration exists and sometimes there is no strict limit between fields,<ref>{{Cite book|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=uV9TZzOITMwC&q=%22biological%20plausibility%22&pg=PA17|title=Neural Network Perspectives on Cognition and Adaptive Robotics|last=Browne|first=A.|date=1997-01-01|publisher=CRC Press|isbn=9780750304559|language=en}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Zorzi|first1=Marco|last2=Testolin|first2=Alberto|last3=Stoianov|first3=Ivilin P.|date=2013-08-20|title=Modeling language and cognition with deep unsupervised learning: a tutorial overview|journal=Frontiers in Psychology|volume=4|pages=515|doi=10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00515|issn=1664-1078|pmc=3747356|pmid=23970869|doi-access=free}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Shai|first1=Adam|last2=Larkum|first2=Matthew Evan|date=2017-12-05|title=Branching into brains|journal=eLife|language=en|volume=6|doi=10.7554/eLife.33066|issn=2050-084X|pmc=5716658|pmid=29205152 |doi-access=free }}</ref> with model abstraction in computational neuroscience depending on research scope and the granularity at which biological entities are analyzed. Models in theoretical neuroscience are aimed at capturing the essential features of the biological system at multiple spatial-temporal scales, from membrane currents, and chemical coupling via [[neural oscillation|network oscillations]], columnar and topographic architecture, nuclei, all the way up to psychological faculties like memory, learning and behavior. These computational models frame hypotheses that can be directly tested by biological or psychological experiments. ==History== The term 'computational neuroscience' was introduced by [[Eric L. Schwartz]], who organized a conference, held in 1985 in [[Carmel, California]], at the request of the Systems Development Foundation to provide a summary of the current status of a field which until that point was referred to by a variety of names, such as neural modeling, brain theory and neural networks. The proceedings of this definitional meeting were published in 1990 as the book ''Computational Neuroscience''.<ref>{{cite book |author=Schwartz, Eric |title=Computational neuroscience |publisher=MIT Press |location=Cambridge, Mass |year=1990 |isbn=978-0-262-19291-0 }}</ref> The first of the annual open international meetings focused on Computational Neuroscience was organized by [[James M. Bower]] and John Miller in [[San Francisco, California]] in 1989.<ref>{{cite book |author=Bower, James M. |title=20 years of Computational neuroscience |publisher=Springer |location=Berlin, Germany |year=2013 |isbn=978-1461414230}}</ref> The first graduate educational program in computational neuroscience was organized as the Computational and Neural Systems Ph.D. program at the [[California Institute of Technology]] in 1985. The early historical roots of the field<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Fan |first1=Xue |last2=Markram |first2=Henry |date=2019 |title=A Brief History of Simulation Neuroscience |journal=Frontiers in Neuroinformatics |volume=13 |page=32 |doi=10.3389/fninf.2019.00032 |doi-access=free |issn=1662-5196 |pmc=6513977 |pmid=31133838}}</ref> can be traced to the work of people including [[Louis Lapicque]], [[Alan Hodgkin|Hodgkin]] & [[Andrew Huxley|Huxley]], [[David H. Hubel|Hubel]] and [[Torsten Wiesel|Wiesel]], and [[David Marr (psychologist)|David Marr]]. Lapicque introduced the [[integrate and fire]] model of the neuron in a seminal article published in 1907,<ref>{{cite journal |author=Lapicque L |title= Recherches quantitatives sur l'excitation électrique des nerfs traitée comme une polarisation |journal=J. Physiol. Pathol. Gen. |volume=9 |pages=620–635 |year=1907}}</ref> a model still popular for [[artificial neural network]]s studies because of its simplicity (see a recent review<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Brunel N, Van Rossum MC |title= Lapicque's 1907 paper: from frogs to integrate-and-fire |journal=Biol. Cybern. |volume=97 |pages=337–339 |year=2007 |pmid=17968583 |doi=10.1007/s00422-007-0190-0 |issue=5–6|s2cid= 17816096 }}</ref>). About 40 years later, [[Alan Hodgkin|Hodgkin]] and [[Andrew Huxley|Huxley]] developed the [[voltage clamp]] and created the first biophysical model of the [[action potential]]. [[David H. Hubel|Hubel]] and [[Torsten Wiesel|Wiesel]] discovered that neurons in the [[primary visual cortex]], the first cortical area to process information coming from the [[retina]], have oriented receptive fields and are organized in columns.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Hubel DH, Wiesel TN |title=Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex |journal=J. Physiol. |volume=160 |pages=106–54 |year=1962 |pmid=14449617 |pmc=1359523 |doi= 10.1113/jphysiol.1962.sp006837|url=http://www.jphysiol.org/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=14449617 |issue=1}}</ref> David Marr's work focused on the interactions between neurons, suggesting computational approaches to the study of how functional groups of neurons within the [[hippocampus]] and [[neocortex]] interact, store, process, and transmit information. Computational modeling of biophysically realistic neurons and dendrites began with the work of [[Wilfrid Rall]], with the first multicompartmental model using [[cable theory]]. ==Major topics== Research in computational neuroscience can be roughly categorized into several lines of inquiry. Most computational neuroscientists collaborate closely with experimentalists in analyzing novel data and synthesizing new models of biological phenomena. ===Single-neuron modeling=== {{main|Biological neuron models}} Even a single neuron has complex biophysical characteristics and can perform computations (e.g.<ref>{{cite journal |author=Forrest MD |title=Intracellular Calcium Dynamics Permit a Purkinje Neuron Model to Perform Toggle and Gain Computations Upon its Inputs. |journal=Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience |volume=8 |pages=86 |year=2014 | doi=10.3389/fncom.2014.00086 |pmid=25191262 |pmc=4138505|doi-access=free }}</ref>). Hodgkin and Huxley's [[Hodgkin–Huxley model|original model]] only employed two voltage-sensitive currents (Voltage sensitive ion channels are glycoprotein molecules which extend through the lipid bilayer, allowing ions to traverse under certain conditions through the axolemma), the fast-acting sodium and the inward-rectifying potassium. Though successful in predicting the timing and qualitative features of the action potential, it nevertheless failed to predict a number of important features such as adaptation and [[Shunting (neurophysiology)|shunting]]. Scientists now believe that there are a wide variety of voltage-sensitive currents, and the implications of the differing dynamics, modulations, and sensitivity of these currents is an important topic of computational neuroscience.<ref>{{cite book |author1=Wu, Samuel Miao-sin |author2=Johnston, Daniel |title=Foundations of cellular neurophysiology |publisher=MIT Press |location=Cambridge, Mass |year=1995 |isbn=978-0-262-10053-3 }}</ref> The computational functions of complex [[dendrites]] are also under intense investigation. There is a large body of literature regarding how different currents interact with geometric properties of neurons.<ref>{{cite book |author=Koch, Christof |title=Biophysics of computation: information processing in single neurons |publisher=Oxford University Press |location=Oxford [Oxfordshire] |year=1999 |isbn=978-0-19-510491-2 }}</ref> There are many software packages, such as [[GENESIS (software)|GENESIS]] and [[Neuron (software)|NEURON]], that allow rapid and systematic ''in silico'' modeling of realistic neurons. [[Blue Brain]], a project founded by [[Henry Markram]] from the [[École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne]], aims to construct a biophysically detailed simulation of a [[cortical column]] on the [[Blue Gene]] [[supercomputer]]. Modeling the richness of biophysical properties on the single-neuron scale can supply mechanisms that serve as the building blocks for network dynamics.<ref>{{cite journal|author=Forrest MD|year=2014|title=Intracellular Calcium Dynamics Permit a Purkinje Neuron Model to Perform Toggle and Gain Computations Upon its Inputs.|journal=Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience|volume=8|pages=86|doi=10.3389/fncom.2014.00086|pmc=4138505|pmid=25191262|doi-access=free}}</ref> However, detailed neuron descriptions are computationally expensive and this computing cost can limit the pursuit of realistic network investigations, where many neurons need to be simulated. As a result, researchers that study large neural circuits typically represent each neuron and synapse with an artificially simple model, ignoring much of the biological detail. Hence there is a drive to produce simplified neuron models that can retain significant biological fidelity at a low computational overhead. Algorithms have been developed to produce faithful, faster running, simplified surrogate neuron models from computationally expensive, detailed neuron models.<ref>{{cite journal |author=Forrest MD |title=Simulation of alcohol action upon a detailed Purkinje neuron model and a simpler surrogate model that runs >400 times faster |journal= BMC Neuroscience | volume=16 |issue=27 |pages=27 | date=April 2015 |doi=10.1186/s12868-015-0162-6 |pmid=25928094 |pmc=4417229 |doi-access=free }}</ref> === Modeling Neuron-glia interactions === Glial cells participate significantly in the regulation of neuronal activity at both the cellular and the network level. Modeling this interaction allows to clarify the [[potassium cycle]],<ref>{{Cite web |title=Dynamics of Ion Fluxes between Neurons, Astrocytes and the Extracellular Space during Neurotransmission |url=https://cyberleninka.ru/article/n/dynamics-of-ion-fluxes-between-neurons-astrocytes-and-the-extracellular-space-during-neurotransmission/viewer |access-date=2023-03-14 |website=cyberleninka.ru}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Sibille |first1=Jérémie |last2=Duc |first2=Khanh Dao |last3=Holcman |first3=David |last4=Rouach |first4=Nathalie |date=2015-03-31 |title=The Neuroglial Potassium Cycle during Neurotransmission: Role of Kir4.1 Channels |journal=PLOS Computational Biology |language=en |volume=11 |issue=3 |pages=e1004137 |doi=10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004137 |issn=1553-7358 |pmc=4380507 |pmid=25826753|bibcode=2015PLSCB..11E4137S |doi-access=free }}</ref> so important for maintaining homeostasis and to prevent epileptic seizures. Modeling reveals the role of glial protrusions that can penetrate in some cases the synaptic cleft to interfere with the synaptic transmission and thus control synaptic communication.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Pannasch |first1=Ulrike |last2=Freche |first2=Dominik |last3=Dallérac |first3=Glenn |last4=Ghézali |first4=Grégory |last5=Escartin |first5=Carole |last6=Ezan |first6=Pascal |last7=Cohen-Salmon |first7=Martine |last8=Benchenane |first8=Karim |last9=Abudara |first9=Veronica |last10=Dufour |first10=Amandine |last11=Lübke |first11=Joachim H. R. |last12=Déglon |first12=Nicole |last13=Knott |first13=Graham |last14=Holcman |first14=David |last15=Rouach |first15=Nathalie |date=April 2014 |title=Connexin 30 sets synaptic strength by controlling astroglial synapse invasion |url=https://www.nature.com/articles/nn.3662 |journal=Nature Neuroscience |language=en |volume=17 |issue=4 |pages=549–558 |doi=10.1038/nn.3662 |pmid=24584052 |s2cid=554918 |issn=1546-1726}}</ref> ===Development, axonal patterning, and guidance=== Computational neuroscience aims to address a wide array of questions, including: How do [[axons]] and [[dendrites]] form during development? How do axons know where to target and how to reach these targets? How do neurons migrate to the proper position in the central and peripheral systems? How do synapses form? We know from [[molecular biology]] that distinct parts of the nervous system release distinct chemical cues, from [[growth factors]] to [[hormones]] that modulate and influence the growth and development of functional connections between neurons. Theoretical investigations into the formation and patterning of synaptic connection and morphology are still nascent. One hypothesis that has recently garnered some attention is the ''minimal wiring hypothesis'', which postulates that the formation of axons and dendrites effectively minimizes resource allocation while maintaining maximal information storage.<ref>{{cite journal|author3-link=Karel Svoboda (scientist) |vauthors=Chklovskii DB, Mel BW, Svoboda K |title=Cortical rewiring and information storage |journal=Nature |volume=431 |issue=7010 |pages=782–8 |date=October 2004|pmid=15483599 |doi=10.1038/nature03012 |bibcode = 2004Natur.431..782C |s2cid=4430167 }}<br/>Review article</ref> ===Sensory processing=== Early models on sensory processing understood within a theoretical framework are credited to [[Horace Barlow]]. Somewhat similar to the minimal wiring hypothesis described in the preceding section, Barlow understood the processing of the early sensory systems to be a form of [[efficient coding hypothesis|efficient coding]], where the neurons encoded information which minimized the number of spikes. Experimental and computational work have since supported this hypothesis in one form or another. For the example of visual processing, efficient coding is manifested in the forms of efficient spatial coding, color coding, temporal/motion coding, stereo coding, and combinations of them.<ref>Zhaoping L. 2014, [https://www.oxfordscholarship.com/view/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199564668.001.0001/acprof-9780199564668-chapter-3 The efficient coding principle ], chapter 3, of the textbook [https://global.oup.com/academic/product/understanding-vision-9780198829362?cc=de&lang=en& Understanding vision: theory, models, and data ]</ref> Further along the visual pathway, even the efficiently coded visual information is too much for the capacity of the information bottleneck, the visual attentional bottleneck.<ref>see visual spational attention https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Visual_spatial_attention</ref> A subsequent theory, [[V1 Saliency Hypothesis|V1 Saliency Hypothesis (V1SH)]], has been developed on exogenous attentional selection of a fraction of visual input for further processing, guided by a bottom-up saliency map in the primary visual cortex.<ref name=Li2002>Li. Z. 2002 [https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S1364661300018179 A saliency map in primary visual cortex] Trends in Cognitive Sciences vol. 6, Pages 9-16, and Zhaoping, L. 2014, [https://www.oxfordscholarship.com/view/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199564668.001.0001/acprof-9780199564668-chapter-5 The V1 hypothesis—creating a bottom-up saliency map for preattentive selection and segmentation] in the book [https://www.oxfordscholarship.com/view/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199564668.001.0001/acprof-9780199564668 Understanding Vision: Theory, Models, and Data]</ref> Current research in sensory processing is divided among a biophysical modeling of different subsystems and a more theoretical modeling of perception. Current models of perception have suggested that the brain performs some form of [[Bayesian approaches to brain function|Bayesian inference]] and integration of different sensory information in generating our perception of the physical world.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Weiss|first1=Yair|last2=Simoncelli|first2=Eero P.|last3=Adelson|first3=Edward H.|title=Motion illusions as optimal percepts|journal=Nature Neuroscience|date=20 May 2002|volume=5|issue=6|pages=598–604|doi=10.1038/nn0602-858|pmid=12021763|s2cid=2777968}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last1=Ernst|first1=Marc O.|last2=Bülthoff|first2=Heinrich H.|title=Merging the senses into a robust percept|journal=Trends in Cognitive Sciences|date=April 2004|volume=8|issue=4|pages=162–169|doi=10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.002|pmid=15050512|citeseerx=10.1.1.299.4638|s2cid=7837073}}</ref> ===Motor control=== Many models of the way the brain controls movement have been developed. This includes models of processing in the brain such as the cerebellum's role for error correction, skill learning in motor cortex and the basal ganglia, or the control of the vestibulo ocular reflex. This also includes many normative models, such as those of the Bayesian or optimal control flavor which are built on the idea that the brain efficiently solves its problems. ===Memory and synaptic plasticity=== {{main|Synaptic plasticity}} Earlier models of [[memory]] are primarily based on the postulates of [[Hebbian learning]]. Biologically relevant models such as [[Hopfield net]] have been developed to address the properties of associative (also known as "content-addressable") style of memory that occur in biological systems. These attempts are primarily focusing on the formation of medium- and [[long-term memory]], localizing in the [[hippocampus]]. One of the major problems in neurophysiological memory is how it is maintained and changed through multiple time scales. Unstable [[synapses]] are easy to train but also prone to stochastic disruption. Stable [[synapses]] forget less easily, but they are also harder to consolidate. It is likely that computational tools will contribute greatly to our understanding of how synapses function and change in relation to external stimulus in the coming decades. ===Behaviors of networks=== Biological neurons are connected to each other in a complex, recurrent fashion. These connections are, unlike most [[artificial neural networks]], sparse and usually specific. It is not known how information is transmitted through such sparsely connected networks, although specific areas of the brain, such as the [[visual cortex]], are understood in some detail.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Olshausen|first1=Bruno A.|last2=Field|first2=David J.|date=1997-12-01|title=Sparse coding with an overcomplete basis set: A strategy employed by V1?|journal=Vision Research|volume=37|issue=23|pages=3311–3325|doi=10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00169-7|pmid=9425546|s2cid=14208692|doi-access=free}}</ref> It is also unknown what the computational functions of these specific connectivity patterns are, if any. The interactions of neurons in a small network can be often reduced to simple models such as the [[Ising model]]. The [[statistical mechanics]] of such simple systems are well-characterized theoretically. Some recent evidence suggests that dynamics of arbitrary neuronal networks can be reduced to pairwise interactions.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Schneidman E, Berry MJ, Segev R, Bialek W |title=Weak pairwise correlations imply strongly correlated network states in a neural population |journal=Nature |volume=440 |issue=7087 |pages=1007–12 |year=2006 |pmid=16625187 |pmc=1785327 |doi=10.1038/nature04701 |bibcode=2006Natur.440.1007S|arxiv = q-bio/0512013 }}</ref> It is not known, however, whether such descriptive dynamics impart any important computational function. With the emergence of [[two-photon microscopy]] and [[calcium imaging]], we now have powerful experimental methods with which to test the new theories regarding neuronal networks. In some cases the complex interactions between ''inhibitory'' and ''excitatory'' neurons can be simplified using [[mean-field theory]], which gives rise to the [[Wilson–Cowan model|population model]] of neural networks.<ref>{{cite journal |author1=Wilson, H. R. |author2=Cowan, J.D. |title=A mathematical theory of the functional dynamics of cortical and thalamic nervous tissue |journal=Kybernetik |volume=13 |issue=2 |pages=55–80 |year=1973 |doi= 10.1007/BF00288786|pmid=4767470 |s2cid=292546 }}</ref> While many neurotheorists prefer such models with reduced complexity, others argue that uncovering structural-functional relations depends on including as much neuronal and network structure as possible. Models of this type are typically built in large simulation platforms like GENESIS or NEURON. There have been some attempts to provide unified methods that bridge and integrate these levels of complexity.<ref>{{cite book |author1=Anderson, Charles H. |author2=Eliasmith, Chris |title=Neural Engineering: Computation, Representation, and Dynamics in Neurobiological Systems (Computational Neuroscience) |publisher=The MIT Press |location=Cambridge, Mass |year=2004 |isbn=978-0-262-55060-4 }}</ref> ===Visual attention, identification, and categorization=== Visual attention can be described as a set of mechanisms that limit some processing to a subset of incoming stimuli.<ref>{{cite book|author1=Marvin M. Chun |author2=Jeremy M. Wolfe |author3=E. B. Goldstein | title=Blackwell Handbook of Sensation and Perception |url=https://archive.org/details/blackwellhandboo00gold |url-access=limited | publisher=Blackwell Publishing Ltd |year=2001 | pages=[https://archive.org/details/blackwellhandboo00gold/page/n284 272]–310 |isbn=978-0-631-20684-2}}</ref> Attentional mechanisms shape what we see and what we can act upon. They allow for concurrent selection of some (preferably, relevant) information and inhibition of other information. In order to have a more concrete specification of the mechanism underlying visual attention and the binding of features, a number of computational models have been proposed aiming to explain psychophysical findings. In general, all models postulate the existence of a saliency or priority map for registering the potentially interesting areas of the retinal input, and a gating mechanism for reducing the amount of incoming visual information, so that the limited computational resources of the brain can handle it.<ref>{{cite book|author1=Edmund Rolls |author2=Gustavo Deco | title=Computational Neuroscience of Vision | publisher=Oxford Scholarship Online | year=2012 | isbn=978-0-198-52488-5}}</ref> An example theory that is being extensively tested behaviorally and physiologically is the [[V1 Saliency Hypothesis]] that a bottom-up saliency map is created in the primary visual cortex to guide attention exogenously.<ref name=Li2002 /> Computational neuroscience provides a mathematical framework for studying the mechanisms involved in brain function and allows complete simulation and prediction of neuropsychological syndromes. ===Cognition, discrimination, and learning=== Computational modeling of higher cognitive functions has only recently{{When|date=February 2016}} begun. Experimental data comes primarily from [[single-unit recording]] in [[primates]]. The [[frontal lobe]] and [[parietal lobe]] function as integrators of information from multiple sensory modalities. There are some tentative ideas regarding how simple mutually inhibitory functional circuits in these areas may carry out biologically relevant computation.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Machens CK, Romo R, Brody CD |title=Flexible control of mutual inhibition: a neural model of two-interval discrimination |journal=Science |volume=307 |issue=5712 |pages=1121–4 |year=2005 |pmid=15718474 |doi=10.1126/science.1104171 |bibcode = 2005Sci...307.1121M |citeseerx=10.1.1.523.4396 |s2cid=45378154 }}</ref> The [[brain]] seems to be able to discriminate and adapt particularly well in certain contexts. For instance, human beings seem to have an enormous capacity for memorizing and [[face perception|recognizing faces]]. One of the key goals of computational neuroscience is to dissect how biological systems carry out these complex computations efficiently and potentially replicate these processes in building intelligent machines. The brain's large-scale organizational principles are illuminated by many fields, including biology, psychology, and clinical practice. [[Integrative neuroscience]] attempts to consolidate these observations through unified descriptive models and databases of behavioral measures and recordings. These are the bases for some quantitative modeling of large-scale brain activity.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Robinson PA, Rennie CJ, Rowe DL, O'Connor SC, Gordon E | title=Multiscale brain modelling | journal=Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B | volume=360 | issue=1457|pages=1043–1050|year=2005|doi=10.1098/rstb.2005.1638 |pmid=16087447 |pmc=1854922 }}</ref> The Computational Representational Understanding of Mind ([[CRUM]]) is another attempt at modeling human cognition through simulated processes like acquired rule-based systems in decision making and the manipulation of visual representations in decision making. ===[[Consciousness]]=== One of the ultimate goals of psychology/neuroscience is to be able to explain the everyday experience of conscious life. [[Francis Crick]], [[Giulio Tononi]] and [[Christof Koch]] made some attempts to formulate consistent frameworks for future work in [[neural correlates of consciousness]] (NCC), though much of the work in this field remains speculative.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Crick F, Koch C |title=A framework for consciousness |journal=Nat. Neurosci. |volume=6 |issue=2 |pages=119–26 |year=2003 |pmid=12555104 |doi=10.1038/nn0203-119|s2cid=13960489 |url= https://zenodo.org/record/852680 }}</ref> ===Computational clinical neuroscience=== [[Computational clinical neuroscience]] is a field that brings together experts in neuroscience, [[neurology]], [[psychiatry]], [[decision sciences]] and computational modeling to quantitatively define and investigate problems in [[neurological disorders|neurological]] and [[mental disorders|psychiatric diseases]], and to train scientists and clinicians that wish to apply these models to diagnosis and treatment.<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Adaszewski | first1=Stanisław | last2=Dukart | first2=Juergen | last3=Kherif | first3=Ferath | last4=Frackowiak | first4=Richard | last5=Draganski | first5=Bogdan | author6=Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative |title=How early can we predict Alzheimer's disease using computational anatomy? |journal=Neurobiol Aging |volume=34 |issue=12 |pages=2815–26 |year=2013 |doi=10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.06.015 |pmid=23890839|s2cid=1025210}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Friston KJ, Stephan KE, Montague R, Dolan RJ |title=Computational psychiatry: the brain as a phantastic organ |journal=Lancet Psychiatry |volume=1 |issue=2 |pages=148–58 |year=2014 |doi=10.1016/S2215-0366(14)70275-5 |pmid=26360579 |s2cid=15504512 }}</ref> === Predictive computational neuroscience === Predictive computational neuroscience is a recent field that combines signal processing, neuroscience, clinical data and machine learning to predict the brain during coma <ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Floyrac |first1=Aymeric |last2=Doumergue |first2=Adrien |last3=Legriel |first3=Stéphane |last4=Deye |first4=Nicolas |last5=Megarbane |first5=Bruno |last6=Richard |first6=Alexandra |last7=Meppiel |first7=Elodie |last8=Masmoudi |first8=Sana |last9=Lozeron |first9=Pierre |last10=Vicaut |first10=Eric |last11=Kubis |first11=Nathalie |last12=Holcman |first12=David |date=2023 |title=Predicting neurological outcome after cardiac arrest by combining computational parameters extracted from standard and deviant responses from auditory evoked potentials |journal=Frontiers in Neuroscience |volume=17 |page=988394 |doi=10.3389/fnins.2023.988394 |pmid=36875664 |pmc=9975713 |issn=1662-453X|doi-access=free }}</ref> or anesthesia.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Sun |first1=Christophe |last2=Holcman |first2=David |date=2022-08-01 |title=Combining transient statistical markers from the EEG signal to predict brain sensitivity to general anesthesia |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S174680942200235X |journal=Biomedical Signal Processing and Control |language=en |volume=77 |pages=103713 |doi=10.1016/j.bspc.2022.103713 |s2cid=248488365 |issn=1746-8094}}</ref> For example, it is possible to anticipate deep brain states using the EEG signal. These states can be used to anticipate hypnotic concentration to administrate to the patient. ===Computational Psychiatry=== [[Computational psychiatry]] is a new emerging field that brings together experts in [[machine learning]], [[neuroscience]], [[neurology]], [[psychiatry]], [[psychology]] to provide an understanding of psychiatric disorders.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Montague|first1=P. Read | last2=Dolan| first2=Raymond J. | last3=Friston|first3=Karl J.|author3-link=Karl Friston|last4=Dayan|first4=Peter|author4-link=Peter Dayan|title=Computational psychiatry|journal=[[Trends in Cognitive Sciences]]|date=14 Dec 2011|volume=16|issue=1|pages=72–80|doi=10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.018|pmid=22177032 |pmc=3556822 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1=Kato | first1=Ayaka | last2=Kunisato | first2=Yoshihiko | last3=Katahira | first3=Kentaro | last4=Okimura | first4=Tsukasa | last5=Yamashita | first5=Yuichi |title=Computational Psychiatry Research Map (CPSYMAP): a new database for visualizing research papers |journal=Frontiers in Psychiatry|volume=11|issue=1360|year=2020 | page=578706 |doi=10.3389/fpsyt.2020.578706| pmid=33343418 | pmc=7746554 | doi-access=free }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1=Huys | first1=Quentin J M | last2=Maia | first2=Tiago V | last3=Frank | first3=Michael J |title=Computational psychiatry as a bridge from neuroscience to clinical applications |journal=Nature Neuroscience |volume=19 |issue=3 |pages=404–413 |year=2016 |doi=10.1038/nn.4238 | pmid=26906507 | pmc=5443409 }}</ref> ==Technology== ===Neuromorphic computing=== {{main|Neuromorphic engineering}} A neuromorphic computer/chip is any device that uses physical artificial neurons (made from silicon) to do computations (See: [[neuromorphic computing]], [[physical neural network]]). One of the advantages of using a [[physical model]] computer such as this is that it takes the computational load of the processor (in the sense that the structural and some of the functional elements don't have to be programmed since they are in hardware). In recent times,<ref>{{cite web |last1=Russell |first1=John |title=Beyond von Neumann, Neuromorphic Computing Steadily Advances |date=21 March 2016 |url=https://www.hpcwire.com/2016/03/21/lacking-breakthrough-neuromorphic-computing-steadily-advance/}}</ref> neuromorphic technology has been used to build supercomputers which are used in international neuroscience collaborations. Examples include the [[Human Brain Project]] [[SpiNNaker]] supercomputer and the BrainScaleS computer.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Calimera|first1=Andrea|last2=Macii|first2=Enrico |last3=Poncino|first3=Massimo |date=2013-08-20|title=The human brain project and neuromorphic computing|journal=Functional Neurology|volume=28|issue=3 |pages=191–196 | doi=10.11138/FNeur/2013.28.3.191|doi-broken-date=1 November 2024 |pmid=24139655 |pmc=3812737 }}</ref> ==See also== {{Div col|colwidth=20em}} *[[Action potential]] *[[Biological neuron models]] *[[Bayesian approaches to brain function|Bayesian brain]] *[[Brain simulation]] *[[Computational anatomy]] *[[Connectomics]] *[[Differentiable programming]] *[[Electrophysiology]] *[[FitzHugh–Nagumo model]] *[[Goldman equation]] *[[Hodgkin–Huxley model]] *[[Information theory]] *[[Mathematical model]] *[[Nonlinear dynamics]] *[[Neural coding]] *[[Neural decoding]] *[[Neural oscillation]] *[[Neuroinformatics]] *[[Neuromimetic intelligence]] *[[Neuroplasticity]] *[[Neurophysiology]] *[[Systems neuroscience]] *[[Mathematical and theoretical biology|Theoretical biology]] *[[Theta model]] {{Div col end}} == References == {{Reflist|2}} == Bibliography == * {{cite journal |author=Chklovskii DB |title=Synaptic connectivity and neuronal morphology: two sides of the same coin |journal=Neuron |volume=43 |issue=5 |pages=609–17 |year=2004 |pmid=15339643 |doi=10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.012 |s2cid=16217065 |doi-access=free }} * {{cite book |author1=Sejnowski, Terrence J. |author-link=Terrence Sejnowski |author2=Churchland, Patricia Smith |title=The computational brain |publisher=[[MIT Press]] |location=Cambridge, Mass |year=1992 |isbn=978-0-262-03188-2 }} * {{ cite book | author1= Gerstner, W. | author2 = Kistler, W. | author3 = Naud, R. | author4 = Paninski, L.| title = Neuronal Dynamics | publisher = Cambridge University Press | location = Cambridge, UK | year = 2014 | isbn = 9781107447615}} * {{cite book |author1=Dayan P. |author-link=Peter Dayan |author2=Abbott, L. F. |author2-link=Larry Abbott |title=Theoretical neuroscience: computational and mathematical modeling of neural systems |publisher=MIT Press |location=Cambridge, Mass |year=2001 |isbn=978-0-262-04199-7 }} * {{cite book |author1=Eliasmith, Chris |author2=Anderson, Charles H. |title=Neural engineering: Representation, computation, and dynamics in neurobiological systems |publisher=[[MIT Press]] |location=Cambridge, Mass |year=2003 |isbn=978-0-262-05071-5 }} * {{cite journal |vauthors=[[Alan Hodgkin|Hodgkin AL]], [[Andrew Huxley|Huxley AF]] |title=A quantitative description of membrane current and its application to conduction and excitation in nerve |journal=J. Physiol. |volume=117 |issue=4 |pages=500–44 |date=28 August 1952|pmid=12991237 |pmc=1392413 |url=http://www.jphysiol.org/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=12991237 |doi=10.1113/jphysiol.1952.sp004764}} * {{cite book |author1=William Bialek |author2=Rieke, Fred |author3=David Warland |author4=Rob de Ruyter van Steveninck |title=Spikes: exploring the neural code |publisher=MIT |location=Cambridge, Mass |year=1999 |isbn=978-0-262-68108-7 |author1-link=William Bialek }} * {{cite book |author=Schutter, Erik de |title=Computational neuroscience: realistic modeling for experimentalists |publisher=CRC |location=Boca Raton |year=2001 |isbn=978-0-8493-2068-2 }} * {{cite book |author1=Sejnowski, Terrence J. |author2=Hemmen, J. L. van |title=23 problems in systems neuroscience |publisher=Oxford University Press |location=Oxford [Oxfordshire] |year=2006 |isbn=978-0-19-514822-0 }} * {{cite book |author1=Michael A. Arbib |author2=Shun-ichi Amari |author3=Prudence H. Arbib | title=The Handbook of Brain Theory and Neural Networks|publisher=The MIT Press|location=Cambridge, Massachusetts |year=2002 |isbn=978-0-262-01197-6}} * {{cite book|author1=Zhaoping|author-link=Li Zhaoping|first=Li |title=Understanding vision: theory, models, and data|publisher=Oxford University Press|location=Oxford, UK|year=2014|isbn=978-0199564668}} ==See also== ===Software=== * [[Brian (software)|BRIAN]], a [[Python (programming language)|Python]] based simulator * [[Budapest Reference Connectome]], web based 3D visualization tool to browse connections in the human brain * [[Emergent (software)|Emergent]], neural simulation software. * [[GENESIS (software)|GENESIS]], a general neural simulation system. * [[NEST (software)|NEST]] is a simulator for spiking neural network models that focuses on the dynamics, size and structure of neural systems rather than on the exact morphology of individual neurons. ==External links== {{Commons category}} ===Journals=== *[https://www.springer.com/mathematics/journal/13408 Journal of Mathematical Neuroscience] *[https://www.springer.com/10827 Journal of Computational Neuroscience] * [http://www.mitpressjournals.org/loi/neco Neural Computation] * [[Cognitive Neurodynamics]] * [http://frontiersin.org/neuroscience/computationalneuroscience/ Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience] * [http://www.ploscompbiol.org/home.action PLoS Computational Biology] * [http://www.frontiersin.org/Journal/specialty.aspx?s=752&name=neuroinformatics&x=y Frontiers in Neuroinformatics] ===Conferences=== * [[Computational and Systems Neuroscience]] (COSYNE) – a computational neuroscience meeting with a systems neuroscience focus. * [http://www.cnsorg.org Annual Computational Neuroscience Meeting (CNS)] – a yearly computational neuroscience meeting. * [http://www.nips.cc Neural Information Processing Systems (NIPS)]– a leading annual conference covering mostly machine learning. * [https://ccneuro.org/ Cognitive Computational Neuroscience (CCN)] – a computational neuroscience meeting focusing on computational models capable of cognitive tasks. * [https://web.archive.org/web/20070309063503/http://www.iccn2007.org/ International Conference on Cognitive Neurodynamics (ICCN)] – a yearly conference. * [https://web.archive.org/web/20080705131550/http://www.icms.org.uk/workshops/mathneuro UK Mathematical Neurosciences Meeting]– a yearly conference, focused on mathematical aspects. * [https://web.archive.org/web/20110429094455/http://www.nncn.de/Aktuelles-en/bernsteinsymposium/Symposium/view?set_language=en Bernstein Conference on Computational Neuroscience (BCCN)]– a yearly computational neuroscience conference ]. * [http://www.areadne.org/index.html AREADNE Conferences]– a biennial meeting that includes theoretical and experimental results. ===Websites=== * [http://www.scholarpedia.org/article/Encyclopedia_of_Computational_Neuroscience Encyclopedia of Computational Neuroscience], part of [[Scholarpedia]], an online expert curated encyclopedia on computational neuroscience and dynamical systems {{Neuroscience}} {{Computational science}} {{DEFAULTSORT:Computational Neuroscience}} [[Category:Computational fields of study]] [[Category:Computational neuroscience]] [[Category:Mathematical and theoretical biology]]
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Pages transcluded onto the current version of this page
(
help
)
:
Template:Citation
(
edit
)
Template:Cite book
(
edit
)
Template:Cite journal
(
edit
)
Template:Cite web
(
edit
)
Template:Commons category
(
edit
)
Template:Computational science
(
edit
)
Template:Div col
(
edit
)
Template:Div col end
(
edit
)
Template:Main
(
edit
)
Template:Neuroscience
(
edit
)
Template:Reflist
(
edit
)
Template:Short description
(
edit
)
Template:When
(
edit
)