Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Equal-loudness contour
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
{{short description|Frequency characteristics of hearing and perceived volume}} {{Use American English|date=June 2020}} [[File:Lindos1.svg|thumb|400px|right|alt=Equal-loudness contours from ISO 226:2003 shown with original ISO standard.|[[International Organization for Standardization|ISO]] equal-loudness contours with frequency in Hz]] An '''equal-loudness contour''' is a measure of [[sound pressure level]], over the [[frequency]] spectrum, for which a listener perceives a constant [[loudness]] when presented with pure steady tones.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Suzuki |first1=YĂ´iti |last2=Takeshima |first2=Hisashi |date=2004 |title=Equal-loudness-level contours for pure tones |journal=The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America |volume=116 |issue=2 |pages=918â933 |doi=10.1121/1.1763601 |issn=0001-4966 |pmid=15376658 |bibcode=2004ASAJ..116..918S |s2cid=15865914 |doi-access=free}}</ref> The unit of measurement for loudness levels is the [[phon]] and is arrived at by reference to equal-loudness contours. By definition, two sine waves of differing frequencies are said to have equal-loudness level measured in phons if they are perceived as equally loud by the average young person without significant hearing impairment. The '''FletcherâMunson curves''' are one of many sets of equal-loudness contours for the human ear, determined experimentally by [[Harvey Fletcher]] and Wilden A. Munson, and reported in a 1933 paper entitled "Loudness, its definition, measurement and calculation" in the ''Journal of the Acoustical Society of America''.<ref>Fletcher, H. and Munson, W. A. "Loudness, its definition, measurement and calculation", ''Journal of the Acoustical Society of America'' 5, 82â108 (1933).</ref> FletcherâMunson curves have been superseded and incorporated into newer standards. The definitive curves are those defined in '''ISO 226''' from the [[International Organization for Standardization]], which are based on a review of modern determinations made in various countries. [[Amplifier]]s often feature a "loudness" button, known technically as [[loudness compensation]], that boosts low and high-frequency components of the sound. These are intended to offset the apparent loudness fall-off at those frequencies, especially at lower volume levels. Boosting these frequencies produces a flatter equal-loudness contour that appears to be louder even at low volume, preventing the perceived sound from being dominated by the mid-frequencies where the ear is most sensitive. == FletcherâMunson curves == [[File:Lindos4.svg|400px|right]] The first research on the topic of how the ear hears different frequencies at different levels was conducted by Fletcher and Munson in 1933. Until recently, it was common to see the term ''FletcherâMunson'' used to refer to equal-loudness contours generally, even though a re-determination was carried out by Robinson and Dadson in 1956, which became the basis for an ISO 226 standard. The generic term ''equal-loudness contours'' is now preferred, of which the FletcherâMunson curves are now a sub-set,<ref>{{cite web |title = Fletcher Munson Curve: The Equal Loudness Contour of Human Hearing | url =https://ledgernote.com/columns/mixing-mastering/fletcher-munson-curve/ |website = Ledger Note | date =16 November 2017 | access-date = November 17, 2017 }}</ref> and especially since a 2003 survey by ISO redefined the curves in a new standard.<ref name="ISO 226">{{citation |url=http://www.nedo.go.jp/itd/grant-e/report/00pdf/is-01e.pdf |title=ISO 226:2003 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20070927210848/http://www.nedo.go.jp/itd/grant-e/report/00pdf/is-01e.pdf |archive-date=September 27, 2007 }}</ref> ==Experimental determination== The [[human]] auditory system is sensitive to frequencies from about 20 [[Hz]] to a maximum of around 20,000 Hz, although the upper hearing limit decreases with age. Within this range, the [[human ear]] is most sensitive between 2 and 5 [[kHz]], largely due to the resonance of the [[ear canal]] and the [[transfer function]] of the [[ossicles]] of the middle ear. Fletcher and Munson first measured equal-loudness contours using [[headphones]] (1933). In their study, test subjects listened to pure tones at various frequencies and over 10 dB increments in stimulus intensity. For each frequency and intensity, the listener also listened to a reference tone at 1000 Hz. Fletcher and Munson adjusted the reference tone until the listener perceived that it had the same loudness as the test tone. Loudness, being a psychological quantity, is difficult to measure, so Fletcher and Munson averaged their results over many test subjects to derive reasonable averages. The lowest equal-loudness contour represents the quietest audible toneâthe ''[[absolute threshold of hearing]]''. The highest contour is the ''[[threshold of pain]]''. Churcher and King carried out a second determination in 1937, but their results and Fletcher and Munson's showed considerable discrepancies over parts of the auditory diagram.<ref>D. W. Robinson et al., [http://www.iop.org/EJ/abstract/0508-3443/7/5/302 "A re-determination of the equal-loudness relations for pure tones"], ''Br. J. Appl. Phys.'' '''7''' (1956), pp. 166â181.</ref> In 1956 [[RobinsonâDadson curves|Robinson and Dadson]] produced a new experimental determination that they believed was more accurate. It became the basis for a standard (ISO 226) that was considered definitive until 2003, when ISO revised the standard on the basis of recent assessments by research groups worldwide. ==Recent revision aimed at more precise determination â ISO 226:2023== Perceived discrepancies between early and more recent determinations led the [[International Organization for Standardization]] (ISO) to revise the standard curves in ISO 226. They did this in response to recommendations in a study coordinated by the Research Institute of Electrical Communication, Tohoku University, Japan. The study produced new curves by combining the results of several studiesâby researchers in Japan, Germany, Denmark, UK, and the US. (Japan was the greatest contributor with about 40% of the data.) This has resulted in the recent acceptance of a new set of curves standardized as ISO 226:2003. The report comments on the surprisingly large differences, and the fact that the original FletcherâMunson contours are in better agreement with recent results than the RobinsonâDadson, which appear to differ by as much as 10â15 dB, especially in the low-frequency region, for reasons not explained.<ref>YĂ´iti Suzuki, et al., [http://www.nedo.go.jp/itd/grant-e/report/00pdf/is-01e.pdf "Precise and Full-range Determination of Two-dimensional Equal Loudness Contours"]. {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20070927210848/http://www.nedo.go.jp/itd/grant-e/report/00pdf/is-01e.pdf |date=2007-09-27 }}.</ref> According to the ISO report, the RobinsonâDadson results were the odd one out, differing more from the current standard than did the FletcherâMunson curves. The report states that it is fortunate that the 40-[[phon]] FletcherâMunson curve on which the [[A-weighting]] standard was based turns out to have been in agreement with modern determinations.<ref name="ISO 226"/> The report also comments on the large differences apparent in the low-frequency region, which remain unexplained. Possible explanations are:<ref name="ISO 226"/> * The equipment used was not properly calibrated. * The criteria used for judging equal loudness at different frequencies had differed. * Subjects were not properly rested for days in advance, or were exposed to loud noise in traveling to the tests, which tensed the [[tensor tympani]] and [[stapedius muscle]]s controlling low-frequency mechanical coupling. ==Side versus frontal presentation== {{Unreferenced section|date=December 2020}} Real-life sounds from a reasonably distant source arrive as planar wavefronts. If the source of sound is directly in front of the listener, then both ears receive equal intensity, but at frequencies above about 1 kHz the sound that enters the ear canal is partially reduced by the [[head shadow]], and also highly dependent on reflection off the [[pinna (anatomy)|pinna]] (outer ear). Off-centre sounds result in increased head masking at one ear, and subtle changes in the effect of the pinna, especially at the other ear. This combined effect of head-masking and pinna reflection is quantified in a set of curves in three-dimensional space referred to as [[head-related transfer function]]s (HRTFs). Frontal presentation is now regarded as preferable when deriving equal-loudness contours, and the latest ISO standard is specifically based on frontal and central presentation. Because no HRTF is involved in normal headphone listening, equal-loudness curves derived using headphones are valid only for the special case of what is called ''side-presentation'', which is not how we normally hear. The RobinsonâDadson determination used [[loudspeaker]]s, and for a long time the difference from the FletcherâMunson curves was explained partly on the basis that the latter used headphones. However, the ISO report actually lists the latter as using ''compensated'' headphones, though it doesn't make clear how RobinsonâDadson achieved ''compensation''. ==Headphones versus loudspeaker testing== {{unreferenced section|date=October 2015}} Good headphones, well sealed to the ear, provide a flat low-frequency pressure response to the ear canal, with low distortion even at high intensities. At low frequencies, the ear is purely pressure-sensitive, and the cavity formed between headphones and ear is too small to introduce modifying resonances. Headphone testing is, therefore, a good way to derive equal-loudness contours below about 500 Hz, though reservations have been expressed about the validity of headphone measurements when determining the actual threshold of hearing, based on the observation that closing off the ear canal produces increased sensitivity to the sound of blood flow within the ear, which the brain appears to mask in normal listening conditions.{{Citation needed|date=October 2008}} At high frequencies, headphone measurement becomes unreliable, and the various resonances of pinnae (outer ears) and ear canals are severely affected by proximity to the headphone cavity. With speakers, the opposite is true. A flat low-frequency response is hard to obtainâexcept in free space high above ground, or in a very large and [[anechoic chamber]] that is free from reflections down to 20 Hz. Until recently,{{when|date=May 2015}} it was not possible to achieve high levels at frequencies down to 20 Hz without high levels of [[total harmonic distortion|harmonic distortion]]. Even today, the best speakers are likely to generate around 1 to 3% of total harmonic distortion, corresponding to 30 to 40 dB below fundamental. This is not good enough, given the steep rise in loudness (rising to as much as 24 dB per octave) with frequency revealed by the equal-loudness curves below about 100 Hz. A good experimenter must ensure that trial subjects really hear the fundamental and not harmonicsâespecially the third harmonic, which is especially strong as a speaker cone's travel becomes limited as its suspension reaches the limit of compliance. A possible way around the problem is to use acoustic filtering, such as by resonant cavity, in the speaker setup. A flat free-field high-frequency response up to 20 kHz, on the other hand, is comparatively easy to achieve with modern speakers on-axis. These effects must be considered when comparing results of various attempts to measure equal-loudness contours. ==Relevance to sound level and noise measurements== The [[A-weighting]] curveâin widespread use for [[noise measurement]]âis said to have been based on the 40-phon FletcherâMunson curve. However, research in the 1960s demonstrated that determinations of equal-loudness made using pure tones are not directly relevant to our perception of noise.<ref>Bauer, B., Torick, E., [https://ieeexplore.ieee.org/document/1161864/;jsessionid=CC0F4DC87EF30C7DA20C05DA854120C6?tp=&arnumber=1161864&isnumber=26056 "Researches in loudness measurement"], ''IEEE Transactions on Audio and Electroacoustics'', Vol. 14:3 (Sep 1966), pp.141â151.</ref> This is because the cochlea in our inner ear analyzes sounds in terms of spectral content, each "hair-cell" responding to a narrow band of frequencies known as a [[critical band]]. The high-frequency bands are wider in absolute terms than the low-frequency bands, and therefore "collect" proportionately more power from a noise source. However, when more than one critical band is stimulated, the signals to the brain add the various bands to produce the impressions of loudness. For these reasons equal-loudness curves derived using noise bands show an upwards tilt above 1 kHz and a downward tilt below 1 kHz when compared to the curves derived using pure tones. Various [[weighting curve]]s were derived in the 1960s, in particular as part of the [[DIN]] 4550 standard for [[audio quality measurement]], which differed from the A-weighting curve, showing more of a peak around 6 kHz. These gave a more meaningful subjective measure of noise on audio equipment, especially on the newly invented [[compact cassette]] tape recorders with [[Dolby]] noise reduction, which were characterized by a noise spectrum dominated by the higher frequencies. [[BBC Research]] conducted listening trials in an attempt to find the best weighting curve and rectifier combination for use when measuring noise in broadcast equipment, examining the various new weighting curves in the context of noise rather than tones, confirming that they were much more valid than A-weighting when attempting to measure the subjective loudness of noise. This work also investigated the response of human hearing to tone-bursts, clicks, [[pink noise]] and a variety of other sounds that, because of their brief impulsive nature, do not give the ear and brain sufficient time to respond. The results were reported in BBC Research Report EL-17 1968/8 entitled ''The Assessment of Noise in Audio Frequency Circuits''. The [[ITU-R 468 noise weighting]] curve, originally proposed in [[ComitĂŠ consultatif international pour la radio|CCIR]] recommendation 468, but later adopted by numerous standards bodies ([[IEC]], [[British Standards Institution|BSI]], [[Japanese Industrial Standards|JIS]], [[ITU]]) was based on the research, and incorporates a special [[quasi-peak detector]] to account for our reduced sensitivity to short bursts and clicks.<ref>Kenâichiro Masaoka, Kazuho Ono, and Setsu Komiyama, [https://www.jstage.jst.go.jp/article/ast/22/1/22_1_35/_article/-char/en "A measurement of equal-loudness level contours for tone burst"], ''Acoustical Science and Technology'', Vol. 22 (2001), No. 1 pp.35â39.</ref> It is widely used by Broadcasters and audio professionals when they measure noise on broadcast paths and audio equipment, so they can subjectively compare equipment types with different noise spectra and characteristics. ==See also== {{div col|colwidth=22em}} * [[A-weighting]] * [[Audio quality measurement]] * [[Audiogram]] * [[dB(A)]] * [[Listener fatigue]] * [[Luminosity function]], the same concept in vision * [[Mel scale]] * [[Pure tone audiometry]] * [[RobinsonâDadson curves]] * [[Sound level meter]] * [[Weighting filter]] {{div col end}} ==Notes== {{reflist}} ==References== {{refbegin}} * ''Audio Engineer's Reference Book'', 2nd Ed., 1999, edited Michael Talbot Smith, Focal Press. * ''An Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing'' 5th ed, Brian C.J. Moore, Elsevier Press. {{refend}} ==External links== * [http://www.iso.org/iso/en/CatalogueDetailPage.CatalogueDetail?CSNUMBER=34222&scopelist=ALL ISO Standard] * [http://www.mp3-tech.org/programmer/docs/IS-01Y-E.pdf Precise and Full-range Determination of Two-dimensional Equal Loudness Contours] * [http://www.sengpielaudio.com/Fletcher-MunsonIsNotRobinson-Dadson.pdf FletcherâMunson is not RobinsonâDadson (PDF)] * [https://web.archive.org/web/20060115001541/http://www.aist.go.jp/aist_e/latest_research/2003/20031114/20031114.html Full Revision of International Standards for Equal-Loudness Level Contours (ISO 226)] * [http://www.phys.unsw.edu.au/~jw/hearing.html Test your hearing â A tool for measuring your equal-loudness contours] * [https://lindos.co.uk/articles/equal-loudness-contours Equal-loudness contour measurements in detail] * [http://scitation.aip.org/getabs/servlet/GetabsServlet?prog=normal&id=JASMAN000110000005002390000001&idtype=cvips&gifs=yes Evaluation of Loudness-level weightings and LLSEL JASA] * [http://www.aes.org/e-lib/browse.cfm?elib=11081 A Model of Loudness Applicable to Time-Varying Sounds AESJ Article] {{ISO standards}} {{Acoustics}} [[Category:Psychoacoustics]] [[Category:Audio engineering]] [[Category:ISO standards]] [[Category:Sound]] [[Category:Acoustics]]
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Pages transcluded onto the current version of this page
(
help
)
:
Template:Acoustics
(
edit
)
Template:Ambox
(
edit
)
Template:Citation
(
edit
)
Template:Citation needed
(
edit
)
Template:Cite journal
(
edit
)
Template:Cite web
(
edit
)
Template:Div col
(
edit
)
Template:Div col end
(
edit
)
Template:ISO standards
(
edit
)
Template:Refbegin
(
edit
)
Template:Refend
(
edit
)
Template:Reflist
(
edit
)
Template:Short description
(
edit
)
Template:Unreferenced
(
edit
)
Template:Unreferenced section
(
edit
)
Template:Use American English
(
edit
)
Template:Webarchive
(
edit
)
Template:When
(
edit
)