Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Ideophone
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
{{distinguish|text=[[idiophone]], a class of musical instruments}} {{Use American English|date = February 2019}} {{Short description|Words evoking ideas of specific sounds or other sensations}} [[File:Jaan! in Hakui.jpg|thumb|A sculpture demonstrating an example of Japanese sound symbolism, "''jaan!''" ({{lang|ja|ジャーン}})]] An '''ideophone''' (also known as a '''mimetic''' or '''expressive''') is a member of the [[word class]] of [[word]]s that depict sensory imagery or sensations,<ref name=dingemanse23>{{cite book |last1=Dingemanse |first1=Mark |editor-last=van Lier |editor-first=Eva |title=Oxford Handbook of Word Classes |date=2023 |publisher=Oxford University Press |location=Oxford |isbn= 9780191887185 |pages=466-476 |doi=10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198852889.013.15 |chapter=Ideophones }}</ref> evoking ideas of action, sound, movement, color, or shape. The class of ideophones is the least common [[syntactic category]] cross-linguistically; it occurs mostly in African, Australian, and [[Amerindian languages]], and sporadically elsewhere. Ideophones resemble [[interjection]]s but are different owing to their special [[Phonetics|phonetic]] or [[Derivational morpheme|derivational]] characteristics, and based on their syntactic function within the sentence. They may include sounds that deviate from the language's phonological system, [[Imitation|imitating]]—often in a repetitive manner—sounds of movement, animal noises, bodily sounds, noises made by tools or machines, and the like.<ref name="Sasse">{{cite book |last=Sasse |first=Hans-Jürgen |title=Syntax--Theory and Analysis: An International Handbook |date=2015 |publisher=De Gruyter |isbn=9783110202762 |editor-last=Kiss and Alexiadou |pages=158–217 |chapter=Syntactic categories and subcategories}}</ref> It is globally the only known word class that does not appear in [[English language|English]]. While English does have ideophonic or onomatopoetic expressions, it does not contain a proper class of ideophones because any English onomatopoeic word can be included in one of the classical categories. For example, ''la-di-da'' functions as an adjective while others, such as ''zigzag'', may function as a verb, adverb or adjective, depending on the clausal context. In the sentence "The rabbit zigzag'''ged''' across the meadow", the verb zigzag takes the past ''-ed'' verb ending. In contrast, the reconstructed example *"The rabbit ''zigzag zigzag'' across the meadow" emulates an ideophone but is not [[idiom (language structure)|idiomatic]] to English.<ref>{{cite web |url=https://linguistics.stackexchange.com/questions/27594/how-do-ideophones-and-onomatopoeia-work-in-english |title=Linguistics. How do ideophones and onomatopoeia work in English? |last= |first= |date= |website=linguistics.stackexchange.com |publisher= |access-date=7 October 2022 |quote=}}</ref> Dictionaries of languages like Japanese, Korean, Xhosa, Yoruba, and Zulu list thousands of ideophones.<ref>{{Cite book|url=http://www.lib.kobe-u.ac.jp/handle_gakui/D1004724|archive-url=https://archive.today/20140104105027/http://www.lib.kobe-u.ac.jp/handle_gakui/D1004724|url-status=dead|archive-date=January 4, 2014|title=A Grammar of Sound-Symbolic Words in Japanese: Theoretical Approaches to Iconic and Lexical Properties of Japanese Mimetics|last=Akita|first=Kimi|publisher=Kobe University|year=2009}}</ref> Sometimes ideophones are called ''phonosemantic'' to indicate that it is not a grammatical word class in the traditional sense of the word (like [[verb]] or [[noun]]), but rather a lexical class based on the special relationship between form and meaning exhibited by ideophones. In the discipline of [[linguistics]], ideophones have sometimes been overlooked or treated as a subgroup of interjections.<ref name="Sasse2">{{cite book |last=Sasse |first=Hans-Jürgen |title=Syntax--Theory and Analysis: An International Handbook |date=2015 |publisher=De Gruyter |isbn=9783110202762 |editor-last=Kiss and Alexiadou |pages=158–217 |chapter=Syntactic categories and subcategories}}</ref> ==Characteristics== The word ideophone was coined in 1935 by [[Clement Martyn Doke]], who defined it in his ''Bantu Linguistic Terminology'' as follows.<ref name=":1">Doke 1935 as cited in Voeltz & Kilian-Hatz 2001</ref> {{quote|A vivid representation of an idea in sound. A word, often onomatopoeic, which describes a predicate, qualificative or adverb in respect to manner, color, sound, smell, action, state or intensity.}} Ideophones evoke sensory events. A well known instance of ideophones are [[onomatopoeia|onomatopoeic]] words—words that imitate the sound (of the event) they refer to. Some ideophones may be derived from onomatopoeic notions. In many languages, however, ideophones do not solely represent sound.<ref name=":1" /> For instance, in [[Gbaya languages|Gbaya]], ''kpuk '''a rap on the door' may be onomatopoeic, but other ideophones depict motion and visual scenes: ''loɓoto-loɓoto'' 'large animals plodding through mud', ''kiláŋ-kiláŋ'' 'in a zigzagging motion', ''pɛɗɛŋ-pɛɗɛŋ'' 'razor sharp'.<ref name=":2" /> Ideophones are often characterized as [[Iconicity|iconic]] or [[Sound symbolism|sound-symbolic]] words, meaning that there can be a resemblance between their form and their meaning. For instance, in West-African languages, voiced consonants and low tone in ideophones are often connected to largeness and heaviness, whereas voiceless consonants and high tones tend to relate to smallness and lightness.<ref>Westermann 1927</ref> [[Reduplication]] figures quite prominently in ideophones, often conveying a sense of repetition or plurality present in the evoked event.<ref>Watson in Voeltz & Kilian-Hatz 2001</ref> The iconicity of ideophones is shown by the fact that people can guess the meanings of ideophones from various languages at a level above chance.<ref>Iwasaki et al. 2007, Dingemanse et al. 2015</ref> However, the form of ideophones does not completely relate to their meaning; as conventionalized words, they contain arbitrary, language-specific phonemes just like other parts of the vocabulary.{{cn|date=January 2025}} ===Grammar=== The grammatical function of ideophones varies by language. In some languages (e.g. [[Welayta language|Welayta]], [[Yir-Yoront language|Yir-Yiront]], [[Semai language|Semai]], [[Korean language|Korean]]), they form a separate word class, while in others, they occur across a number of different word classes (e.g. [[Mundang]], [[Ewe language|Ewe]], [[Sotho language|Sotho]], [[Hausa language|Hausa]]).<ref>Voeltz & Kilian-Hatz 2001</ref> Despite this diversity, ideophones show a number of robust regularities across languages. One is that they are often marked in the same way as quoted speech and demonstrations.<ref name=":3">{{Cite journal|last=Samarin|first=William J.|year=1971|title=Survey of Bantu ideophones|journal=African Language Studies|volume=12|pages=130–168}}</ref> Sometimes ideophones can form a complete utterance on their own, as in English "ta-da!" or Japanese {{Nihongo||ジャーン|jaan|ta-da}}.<ref name=":0">Diffloth 1972</ref> However, in such cases the word ideophone is used as a synonym to interjection. Proper ideophones may occur within utterances, depicting a scene described by other elements of the utterance, as in Japanese ''Taro wa <u>sutasuta to</u> haya-aruki o shita'' "Taro walked hurriedly' (literally 'Taro did haste-walk ''sutasuta''<nowiki/>').<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Kita|first=Sotaro|year=1997|title=Two-dimensional semantic analysis of Japanese mimetics|url=http://wrap.warwick.ac.uk/106489/9/WRAP-two-dimensional-semantic-analysis-Japanese-mimetics-Kita-2018.pdf|journal=Linguistics|volume=35|issue=2|pages=379–415|doi=10.1515/ling.1997.35.2.379|s2cid=144380452 }}</ref> Ideophones are more like illustrations ''of'' events than responses ''to'' events. An ideophone like Gbaya ''kiláŋ-kiláŋ'' 'in a zigzagging motion' displays a certain resemblance to the event (for instance, its irregular vowels and tones depicting the irregularity of the motion).{{cn|date=January 2025}} ===Registers=== Languages may differ in the context in which ideophones are used. In some languages, ideophones are primarily used in spoken language (e.g. [[narrative]] contexts) and are rarely encountered in written language.<ref name=":2">Noss in Voeltz & Kilian-Hatz 2001</ref> In other languages (e.g. [[Ewe language|Ewe]], [[Japanese language|Japanese]]), ideophones can be freely used in all [[Register (sociolinguistics)|registers]]. In general, however, ideophones tend to occur more extensively in spoken language because of their expressive or dramaturgic function.<ref name="ReferenceA">Kunene in Voeltz & Kilian-Hatz 2001</ref> ==Examples== {{No citations section|date=April 2025}} ===[[Japanese language|Japanese]]=== {{main|Japanese sound symbolism}} The Japanese language has thousands of ideophones, often called ''mimetics''. The constructions are quite metrical 2-2, or 3-3, where [[Mora (linguistics)|morae]] play a role in the symmetry. The first consonant of the second word of the reduplication [[rendaku|may become voiced]] if phonological conditions allow. Japanese ideophones are used extensively in daily conversations as well as in the written language.{{cn|date=January 2025}} * ''doki doki'' ({{lang|ja|ドキドキ}}) – heart-pounding * ''kira kira'' ({{lang|ja|キラキラ}}) – glittery * ''shiin'' ({{lang|ja|シーン}}) – silence * ''niko niko'' ({{lang|ja|ニコニコ}}) – smile * ''jii'' ({{lang|ja|じー}}) – stare * ''run run'' ({{lang|ja|ルンルン}}) – cheerful ===[[Tamil language|Tamil]]=== The Tamil language uses many ideophones, both in spoken (colloquial) and in formal usage. Ideophones are called irattaik kilavi (இரட்டைக் கிளவி) in Tamil grammar.{{cn|date=January 2025}} * ''sora sora'' ({{lang|ta|சொறசொற}}) – rough (the sound produced when rubbing back and forth on a rough surface) * ''vazha-vazha'' ({{lang|ta|வழவழ}}) – smooth, slippery * ''mozhu-mozhu'' ({{lang|ta|மொழுமொழு}}) – smooth (surface) * ''kozhu-kozhu'' ({{lang|ta|கொழுகொழு}}) – plump * ''kozha-kozha'' ({{lang|ta|கொழகொழ}}) – slimy, gooey * ''busu-busu'' ({{lang|ta|புசுபுசு}}) – soft and bushy * ''kaNa-kaNa'' ({{lang|ta|கணகண}}) - warm, hot * ''giDu-giDu'' ({{lang|ta|கிடுகிடு}}) – quickly, fast * ''Tak-Tak'' ({{lang|ta|டக்டக்}}) - quickly, rapidly * ''maDa-maDa'' ({{lang|ta|மடமட}}) – quickly, fast * ''masa-masa'' ({{lang|ta|மசமச}}) – sluggish, lethargic * ''viru-viru'' ({{lang|ta|விறுவிறு}}) – energetically (also, spicy) * ''choda-choda'' ({{lang|ta|சொதசொத}}) – marshy, waterlogged * ''paLa-paLa'' ({{lang|ta|பளப்பள/பளபள}}) – glittering, shiny * ''veDa-veDa'' ({{lang|ta|வெடவெட}}) – shaking, trembling * ''chuDa-chuDa'' ({{lang|ta|சுடச்சுட}}) – piping hot * ''mAngu-mAngu'' ({{lang|ta|மாங்குமாங்கு}}) – laboriously * ''gara-gara'' ({{lang|ta|கரகர}}) – crunchy (as in food), gravely (as in voice) * ''gaba-gaba'' ({{lang|ta|கபகப}}) - wolfing down food * ''doLa-doLa'' ({{lang|ta|தொளதொள}}) - hanging loose (as in loose fitting) * ''taLa-taLa'' ({{lang|ta|தளதள}}) - lush (as in a lush plant/orchard) * ''toNa-toNa'' ({{lang|ta|தொணதொண}}) - annoyingly incessant ===Xhosa=== In [[Xhosa language|Xhosa]], as in closely related Zulu, ideophones can convey very complex experiential impressions or can just strengthen meanings of other words. The ideophone is often introduced using the verb {{lang|xh|thi}} {{gloss|say}}.{{cn|date=January 2025}} Using {{lang|xh|thi}}: * {{lang|xh|cwaka}} – to be silent :: {{lang|xh|Lixesha lokuthi cwaka}}. 'It is time to be silent.' [literally: 'It is time to say {{lang|xh|cwaka}}.'] * {{lang|xh|gqi}} – to suddenly appear :: {{lang|xh|Bathi gqi abelungu eAfrika}}. 'The white people suddenly arrived in Africa.' [Literally: 'The white people said {{lang|xh|gqi}} in Africa.']) Without using {{lang|xh|thi}}: * {{lang|xh|ncam}} – exact {{interlinear|indent=3|lang=xh|Andazi '''ncam'''.|{I do not know} IDEO|'I do not know exactly.'}} * ''bhuxe'' – to stand motionless {{interlinear|indent=3|lang=xh|Ndime '''bhuxe''' phambi kwakhe.|{I stood} IDEO before him.|'I stood motionless before him.'}} ==See also== * [[Ideasthesia]] * [[Sound symbolism]] (phonosemantics) * [[Synesthesia]] * [[Reduplication]] * [[Onomatopoeia]] * [[Japanese sound symbolism]] * [[Bouba/kiki effect]] ==References== {{Reflist}} ==Bibliography== {{refbegin}} * {{Cite book| publisher = Cambridge University Press| isbn = 0-521-45219-8| pages = [https://archive.org/details/soundsymbolism0000unse/page/178 178–204]|editor1= Leanne Hinton |editor2=Johanna Nichols |editor3=John J. Ohala | last = Childs| first = G. Tucker| title = Sound Symbolism| chapter = African Ideophones| location = Cambridge| date = 1994| chapter-url = https://archive.org/details/soundsymbolism0000unse/page/178}} * {{Cite journal| volume = 8| pages = 440–447| last = Diffloth| first = Gérard| title = Notes on expressive meaning| journal = Chicago Linguistic Society| date = 1972}} * {{Cite journal| doi = 10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.013| volume = 19| issue = 10| pages = 603–615| last1 = Dingemanse| first1 = Mark| last2 = Blasi| first2 = Damián E.| last3 = Lupyan| first3 = Gary| last4 = Christiansen| first4 = Morten H.| last5 = Monaghan| first5 = Padraic| title = Arbitrariness, iconicity and systematicity in language| journal = Trends in Cognitive Sciences| date = 2015| pmid=26412098| hdl = 11858/00-001M-0000-0028-7AAB-C| s2cid = 3634642| hdl-access = free| author1-link = Mark Dingemanse}} * {{Cite journal| doi = 10.1353/lan.2016.0034| volume = 92| issue = 2| pages = e117–e133| last1 = Dingemanse | first1 = Mark| last2 = Schuerman| first2 = Will| last3 = Reinisch| first3 = Eva| last4 = Tufvesson| first4 = Sylvia| last5 = Mitterer| first5 = Holger| title = What sound symbolism can and cannot do: testing the iconicity of ideophones from five languages| journal = Language| date = 2016| hdl = 11858/00-001M-0000-002A-62F0-7| s2cid = 148546000| hdl-access = free}} * {{cite journal |last=Ido |first=Shinji |date= 2011 |title= Vowel alternation in disyllabic reduplicatives: An areal dimension|url=http://ojs.utlib.ee/index.php/jeful/article/view/jeful.2011.2.1.12/10282 |journal= Eesti ja Soome-Ugri Keeleteaduse Ajakiri |volume=2 |issue= 1 |pages=185–193}} * {{Cite journal| doi = 10.1098/rstb.2013.0298| issn = 0962-8436| volume = 369| issue = 1651| pages = 20130298| last1 = Imai| first1 = Mutsumi| last2 = Kita| first2 = Sotaro| title = The sound symbolism bootstrapping hypothesis for language acquisition and language evolution| journal = Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences| date = 2014-09-19| pmid = 25092666| pmc=4123677}} * {{Cite journal| volume = 17| pages = 53–78| last1 = Iwasaki| first1 = Noriko| last2 = Vinson| first2 = David P.| last3 = Vigliocco| first3 = Gabriella| title = What do English Speakers know about gera-gera and yota-yota?: A Cross-linguistic Investigation of Mimetic Words for Laughing and Walking| journal = Japanese Language Education Around the Globe| date = 2007}} * {{Cite book| publisher = University of Texas| pages = 131–142|editor1=Wai Fong Chiang |editor2=Elaine Chun |editor3=Laura Mahalingappa |editor4=Siri Mehus | last = Nuckolls| first = Janis B.| title = SALSA XI: Proceedings of the Eleventh Annual Symposium about Language and Society| chapter = To be or to be not ideophonically impoverished| location = Austin| series = Texas Linguistic Forum| date = 2004}} * {{Cite book| publisher = John Benjamins| isbn = 978-90-272-2946-5|editor1= Voeltz, F. K. Erhard |editor2=Christa Kilian-Hatz | title = Ideophones| location = Amsterdam| series = Typological Studies in Language| date = 2001|url = https://vdocuments.mx/ideophones-typological-studies-in-language-44.html}} * {{Cite book| publisher = L. Friederichsen| pages = 315–328|editor1= Boas, Franz | last = Westermann| first = Diedrich Hermann| title = Festschrift Meinhof| chapter = Laut, Ton und Sinn in westafrikanischen Sudansprachen| location = Hamburg| date = 1927}} {{refend}} [[Category:Phonaesthetics]] [[Category:Semantics]] [[Category:Parts of speech]]
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Pages transcluded onto the current version of this page
(
help
)
:
Template:Cite book
(
edit
)
Template:Cite journal
(
edit
)
Template:Cite web
(
edit
)
Template:Cn
(
edit
)
Template:Distinguish
(
edit
)
Template:Gloss
(
edit
)
Template:Interlinear
(
edit
)
Template:Lang
(
edit
)
Template:Main
(
edit
)
Template:Nihongo
(
edit
)
Template:No citations section
(
edit
)
Template:Quote
(
edit
)
Template:Refbegin
(
edit
)
Template:Refend
(
edit
)
Template:Reflist
(
edit
)
Template:Short description
(
edit
)
Template:Use American English
(
edit
)