Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Kruithof curve
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
{{Short description|Region of color temperatures that are often viewed as pleasing to an observer}} {{Use American English|date=March 2021}} {{Use mdy dates|date=March 2021}} [[Image:Kruithof curve 2.svg|right|400px|thumb|The Kruithof curve, with an example light source; [[CIE Standard Illuminant D65|D65]] (Northern [[daylight]]), inside the pleasing region.<ref>{{Cite journal|url=http://palimpsest.stanford.edu/waac/wn/wn21/wn21-3/wn21-308.html|title=The Color of White: Is there a "preferred" color temperature for the exhibition of works of art?|date=September 2000|volume=21|issue=3|first=Steven|last=Weintraub|journal=Western Association for Art Conservation Newsletter}}</ref>]] The '''Kruithof curve''' describes a region of [[illuminance]] levels and [[color temperature]]s that are often viewed as comfortable or pleasing to an observer. The curve was constructed from [[Psychophysics|psychophysical]] data collected by Dutch physicist [[Arie Andries Kruithof]],<ref>{{Cite web|first=Arie Andries|last=Kruithof|url=http://dap.library.uu.nl/cgi-bin/dap/dap?diss_id=7789|title=Aanslag van het waterstofmolecuulspectrum door electronen|date=December 12, 1934|access-date=May 6, 2008|archive-date=July 24, 2011|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110724171918/http://dap.library.uu.nl/cgi-bin/dap/dap?diss_id=7789|url-status=dead}} (PhD dissertation at [[Utrecht University]] under [[Leonard Ornstein]]) {{in lang|nl}}</ref> though the original experimental data is not present on the curve itself. Lighting conditions within the bounded region were [[Empirical evidence|empirically]] assessed as being pleasing or natural, whereas conditions outside the region were considered uncomfortable, displeasing or unnatural.<ref name=Kruithof1941>{{Cite journal|last=Kruithof|first=Arie Andries|title=Tubular Luminescence Lamps for General Illumination |journal=Philips Technical Review| volume=6|issue=3|pages=65–96|year=1941|issn=0031-7926}}</ref> The Kruithof curve is a sufficient model for describing [[Light#Light source|sources]] that are considered [[Sunlight|natural]] or closely resemble [[Planck's law|Planckian]] [[black body|black bodies]], but its value in describing human preference has been consistently questioned by further studies on interior lighting.<ref name="Davis & Ginthner 1990"/><ref name="Boyce & Cuttle 1990"/> For example, natural daylight has a color temperature of 6500 [[Kelvin|K]] and an illuminance of about 10<sup>4</sup> to 10<sup>5</sup> [[lux]].{{Citation needed|date=February 2025}} This color temperature–illuminance pair results in natural [[Color Rendering Index|color rendition]], but if viewed at a low illuminance, would appear bluish. At typical indoor office illuminance levels of about 400 lux,{{Citation needed|date=February 2025}} pleasing color temperatures are lower (between 3000 and 6000 K), and at typical home illuminance levels of about 75 lux,{{Citation needed|date=February 2025}} pleasing color temperatures are even lower (between 2400 and 2700 K). These color temperature-illuminance pairs are often achieved with [[Fluorescent lamp|fluorescent]] and [[Incandescent light bulb|incandescent sources]], respectively. The pleasing region of the curve contains color temperatures and illuminance levels comparable to naturally lit environments. ==History== At the emergence of fluorescent lighting in 1941, Kruithof conducted psychophysical experiments to provide a technical guide to design artificial [[lighting]].<ref>{{cite journal|last=Viénot|first=Françoise |author2=Marie-Lucie Durand |author3=Elodie Mahler|title=Kruithof's rule revisited using LED illumination|journal=Journal of Modern Optics|date=20 July 2009|volume=56|issue=13|pages=1433–1446|doi=10.1080/09500340903151278|bibcode=2009JMOp...56.1433V |s2cid=121921684 }}</ref> Using [[Gas-discharge lamp|gas-discharge fluorescent lamps]], Kruithof was able to manipulate the color of emitted light and ask observers to report as to whether or not the source was pleasing to them. The sketch of his curve as presented consists of three major regions: the middle region, which corresponds to light sources considered pleasing; the lower region, which corresponds to colors that are considered cold and dim; and the upper region, which corresponds to colors that are warm and unnaturally colorful. These regions, while approximate, are still used to determine appropriate lighting configurations for homes or offices. ==Perception and adaptation== [[File:Red geranium photopic mesopic scotopic.jpg|thumb|right|Simulated appearance of a red geranium and foliage in normal bright-light ([[Photopic vision|photopic]]) vision, dusk ([[Mesopic vision|mesopic]]) vision, and night ([[Scotopic vision|scotopic]]) vision. The blueish flower centers are still [[Perception|perceived]] as bright in the image of the flower viewed at dusk and at night.]] Kruithof's findings are directly related to human [[Adaptation (eye)|adaptation]] to changes in illumination. As illuminance decreases, human [[Spectral sensitivity|sensitivity]] to blue light increases. This is known as the [[Purkinje effect]].<ref name="frisby1980">{{Cite book | author=Frisby, John P. | title=Seeing: Illusion, Brain and Mind | publisher=Oxford University Press | location=Oxford | year=1980 | isbn=978-0-19-217672-1 | url-access=registration | url=https://archive.org/details/seeingillusionbr0000fris }} </ref> The human [[visual system]] switches from photopic ([[Cone cell|cone]]-dominated) vision to scotopic ([[Rod cell|rod]]-dominated) vision when [[luminance]] levels decrease. Rods have a very high spectral sensitivity to blue energy, whereas cones have varying spectral sensitivities to reds, greens and blues. Since the dominating [[Photoreceptor cell|photoreceptor]] in scotopic vision is most sensitive to blue, human sensitivity to blue light is therefore increased. Because of this, intense sources of higher (bluer) color temperatures are all generally considered to be displeasing at low luminance levels, and a narrow range of pleasing sources exist. Subsequently, the range of pleasing sources increases in photopic vision as luminance levels are increased. ==Criticism== While the curve has been used as a guide to design artificial lighting for indoor spaces, with the general suggestion to use sources with low correlated color temperatures (CCT) at low illuminances,<ref>{{cite book|last=Boyce|first=Peter R.|title=Human factors in lighting|year=2003|publisher=Taylor & Francis|location=London|isbn=978-0-7484-0950-1|pages=245–250|edition=2|chapter=Lighting for offices}}</ref> Kruithof did not describe the method of evaluation, the independent variables, nor the test sample that were used to develop the curve. Without these data, nor other validation, the conclusions should not be considered credible. The relationship between illuminance and CCT was not supported by subsequent work.<ref name="Boyce & Cuttle 1990">{{cite journal |last1=Boyce |first1=P.R. |last2=Cuttle |first2=C. |date=1990 |title=Effect of correlated colour temperature on the perception of interiors and colour discrimination |journal=[[Lighting Research & Technology]] |volume=22 |issue=1 |pages=19–36 |doi=10.1177/096032719002200102 |doi-access=free}}</ref><ref name="Davis & Ginthner 1990">{{cite journal|last1=Davis|first1=RG|last2=Ginthner|first2=DN|title=Correlated color temperature, illuminance level and the Kruithof curve|journal=Journal of the Illuminating Engineering Society|date=1990|volume=Winter|pages=27–38|doi=10.1080/00994480.1990.10747937}}</ref> Illuminance and CCT has been examined in many studies of interior lighting <ref name="Davis & Ginthner 1990" /><ref name="Boyce & Cuttle 1990" /><ref>{{cite journal|last1=Vienot|first1=F|last2=Durand|first2=M|last3=Mahler|first3=E|title=Kruithof's rule revisited using LED illumination|journal=Journal of Modern Optics|date=2009|volume=56|issue=13|pages=1433–1466|doi=10.1080/09500340903151278|bibcode=2009JMOp...56.1433V|s2cid=121921684}}</ref><ref name="Islam et al 2015">{{cite journal|last1=Islam|first1=MS|last2=Dangol|first2=R|last3=Hyvärinen|first3=M|last4=Bhusal|first4=P|last5=Ouolakka|first5=M|last6=Halonen|first6=L|title=User acceptance studies for LED office lighting: Lamp spectrum, spatial brightness and illuminance|journal=Lighting Research and Technology|date=2015|volume=47|pages=54–79|doi=10.1177/1477153513514425|s2cid=109592929 }}</ref><ref name="Wei et al 2014">{{cite journal|last1=Wei|first1=Minchen|last2=Houser|first2=Kevin W.|last3=Orland|first3=Brian|last4=Lang|first4=Dean H.|last5=Ram|first5=Nilam|last6=Sliwinskiwinski|first6=Martin J.|last7=Bose|first7=Mallika|title=Field study of office worker responses to fluorescent lighting of different CCT and lumen output|journal=Journal of Environmental Psychology|volume=39|pages=62–76|doi=10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.04.009|year=2014}}</ref> and these studies consistently demonstrate a different relationship to that suggested by Kruithof.<ref name="Fotios 2017">{{cite journal|last1=Fotios|first1=Steve|title=A Revised Kruithof Graph Based on Empirical Data|journal=LEUKOS|date=2 January 2017|volume=13|issue=1|pages=3–17|doi=10.1080/15502724.2016.1159137|issn=1550-2724|doi-access=free}}</ref> Rather than having upper and lower boundaries, these studies do not suggest CCT to have significant effect and for illuminance suggest only to avoid levels below 300 lux. Current studies have not explored the main critical part that is the low illumination regime or the low CCT range beneath 3000K in general, though some of the studies above reached down to 2850K. This lacunae in the data is particularly important as it relates to almost all "lifestyle" environments in which lighting designers operate - hotels, restaurants and residential settings. Further evaluation of these areas would serve well, given the implications for recent learning on [[Light effects on circadian rhythm | the health implications of light on the circadian rhythm]]. ==Further studies== The Kruithof curve, as presented, does not contain experimental data points and serves as an approximation for desirable lighting conditions. Therefore, its scientific accuracy has been reassessed. Color rendering index is a [[Units of measurement|metric]] for describing the [[Visual appearance|appearance]] of a source and whether or not it is considered pleasing. The color rendering index of a given source is a measure of that source's ability to faithfully reproduce colors of an object. Light sources, like [[candle]]s or incandescent light bulbs produce spectrums of [[Electromagnetic spectrum|electromagnetic]] energy that closely resemble Planckian black bodies; they look much like natural sources. Many fluorescent lamps or [[LED lamp|LED light bulbs]] have spectrums that do not match those of Planckian blackbodies and are considered unnatural. Therefore, the way that they render the perceived colors of an environment may be also considered unnatural. While these newer sources can still achieve correlated color temperatures and illuminance levels that are within the comfortable region of the Kruithof curve, variability in their color rendering indices may cause these sources to ultimately be displeasing. Different activities or scenarios call for different color temperature–illuminance pairs: preferred light sources change depending on the scenario the source is illuminating.<ref>{{cite tech report|url=http://naoyuki-oi.la.coocan.jp/Research/CIE_2007_proceeding.pdf|first=Naoyuki|last=Oi|author2=Hironobu Takahashi|title=Preferred Combinations Between Illuminance and Color Temperature in Several Settings for Daily Living Activities|institution=Kyushu University|year=2007}}</ref> Individuals did prefer color temperature–illuminance pairs within the comfortable region for [[Eating|dining]], [[Socialization|socializing]] and [[Study skills|studying]], but also preferred color temperature–illuminance pairs that were in the lower uncomfortable region for night time activities and preparing for bed. This is linked to the Purkinje effect; individuals who desire some light at night time desire lower (redder) color temperatures even if luminance levels are very low. Kruithof's findings may also vary as a function of [[culture]] or [[Geography|geographic]] location. Desirable sources are based on an individual's previous experiences of perceiving color, and as different regions of the world may have their own lighting standards, each culture would likely have its own acceptable light sources. The illuminance of a source is the dominating factor for deciding as to whether or not a source is pleasing or comfortable, as viewers participating in this experiment evaluated a range of correlated color temperatures and illuminance levels, yet their impressions remained generally unchanged as correlated color temperature changed.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Bodmann|first=H.W.|author2=G. Sollner |author3=E. Voit |title=Evaluation of lighting level with various kinds of light|journal=Proceedings of the CIE|year=1963|volume=15}}</ref> Additionally, there is a relationship between correlated color temperature and apparent brightness of a source.<ref>{{cite thesis|last=Han|first=S.|title=Effect of Illuminance, CCT and Decor on the Perception of Lighting|degree=M.S.|location=[[Troy, New York]]|institution = [[Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute]]}}</ref> From these findings, it is evident that color rendering index, in place of correlated color temperature, may be a more appropriate metric for determining as to whether or not a certain source is considered pleasing. ==See also== * [[Melanopsin]] * [[Melatonin]] ==References== {{reflist}} ==Further reading== * {{Cite journal|author=Robert G. Davis |author2=Dolores N. Ginthner|year=1990|title=Correlated color temperature, illuminance level, and the Kruithof curve|journal=Journal of the Illuminating Engineering Society|volume=19|issue=1|pages=27–38|doi=10.1080/00994480.1990.10747937}} * {{Cite journal|journal=[[Journal of the Optical Society of America A]]|title=Correlated color temperature preferred by observers for illumination of artistic paintings|author1=Paulo Daniel Pinto |author2=João Manuel Maciel Linhares |author3=Sérgio Miguel Cardoso Nascimento |date=March 2008|pages=623–630|volume=25|issue=3| url=http://www.arauto.uminho.pt/pessoas/smcn/pdf/PPinto%20josa2008%20early%20posting.pdf|doi=10.1364/JOSAA.25.000623 |pmid=18311230 |bibcode=2008JOSAA..25..623P }} (A study in which the average luminance was 8 [[candela|cd]]/m<sup>2</sup>, or the illumination 200–400 [[lux]], with an average of about 330 lux.) ==External links== * [https://web.archive.org/web/19971221105330/http://soluxtli.com/edu13.htm Daylight: Is it in the eye of the beholder?] by Kevin P. McGuire. {{color topics}} {{DEFAULTSORT:Kruithof Curve}} [[Category:Color]] [[Category:Vision]] [[Category:Lighting]] [[Category:Psychophysics]]
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Pages transcluded onto the current version of this page
(
help
)
:
Template:Citation needed
(
edit
)
Template:Cite book
(
edit
)
Template:Cite journal
(
edit
)
Template:Cite tech report
(
edit
)
Template:Cite thesis
(
edit
)
Template:Cite web
(
edit
)
Template:Color topics
(
edit
)
Template:In lang
(
edit
)
Template:Reflist
(
edit
)
Template:Short description
(
edit
)
Template:Use American English
(
edit
)
Template:Use mdy dates
(
edit
)