Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
News values
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
{{Short description|Criteria that influence the selection of events as published news}} {{Use dmy dates|date=May 2024}} {{Redirect|Newsworthy|the New Zealand television programme|Newsworthy (TV programme)}} {{Journalism sidebar}} '''News values''' are "criteria that influence the selection and presentation of events as published news." These values help explain what makes something "newsworthy."<ref>{{cite book | last=Boyd | first=Andrew | year=1994 | title=Broadcast journalism: Techniques of radio and TV news | edition=3rd | publication-place=Oxford, UK | publisher=Focal Press | isbn=978-0-7506-1760-4 | oclc=32855888}}</ref> News values are not universal and can vary between different [[culture]]s.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Patrick |first=Rhianna |last2=McLaughlin |first2=Julie |last3=King-Smith |first3=Leah |last4=Schultz |first4=Tristan |last5=Thomson |first5=Tj |last6=Dezuanni |first6=Michael |date=2024-11-29 |title=Unlearning journalism through decolonising, Indigenous approaches |url=https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/14648849241305363 |journal=Journalism |language=en |doi=10.1177/14648849241305363 |issn=1464-8849|doi-access=free }}</ref> Among the many lists of news values that have been drawn up by [[scholar]]s and [[journalist]]s, some attempt to describe news practices across cultures, while others have become remarkably specific to the press of particular (often [[Western world|Western]]) nations. In the Western tradition, decisions on the selection and prioritization of news are made by [[editors]] on the basis of their experience and intuition, although analysis by Johan Galtung and Mari Holmboe Ruge showed that several factors are consistently applied across a range of [[news organizations]]. Their theory was tested on the news presented in four different [[Norway|Norwegian]] newspapers from the [[Congo Crisis|Congo]] and [[Aftermath of the Cuban Revolution|Cuban crisis]] of July 1960 and the [[Cyprus crisis of 1963–1964|Cyprus crisis]] of March–April 1964. Results were mainly consistent with their theory and hypotheses.<ref name="Galtung Holmboe Ruge 1965">{{cite journal | last1=Galtung | first1=Johan | last2=Holmboe Ruge | first2=Mari | year=1965 | title=The structure of foreign news: The presentation of the Congo, Cuba and Cyprus crises in four Norwegian newspapers | journal=[[Journal of Peace Research]] | volume=2 | issue=1 | pages=64–91 | jstor=423011 | doi=10.1177/002234336500200104 | s2cid=55063363}}</ref> Galtung later said that the media have misconstrued his work and become far too negative, sensational, and adversarial.<ref>{{cite news | last=Haagerup | first=Ulrik | date=18 January 2019 | title=Academic who defined news principles says journalists are too negative | work=The Guardian | url=https://www.theguardian.com/world/2019/jan/18/johan-galtung-news-principles-journalists-too-negative | access-date=27 May 2024}}</ref> Methodologically and conceptually, news values can be approached from four different perspectives: material (focusing on the material reality of events), cognitive (focusing on people's beliefs and value systems), social (focusing on [[journalistic]] practice), and discursive (focusing on the discourse).<ref name="Bednarek Caple 2017" /> A discursive perspective tries to systematically examine how news values such as negativity, proximity, eliteness, and others, are constructed through words and images in published [[news story|news stories]]. This approach is influenced by [[linguistics]] and [[social semiotics]], and is called "discursive news values analysis" (DNVA).<ref>{{cite web | url=http://www.newsvaluesanalysis.com/ | title=Discursive News Values Analysis}}</ref>{{better source needed|date=May 2024}} It focuses on the "distortion" step in Galtung and Ruge's chain of news communication, by analysing how events are discursively constructed as newsworthy. == History == Initially labelled "news factors," news values are widely credited to [[Johan Galtung]] and Mari Holmboe Ruge.<ref name="Galtung Holmboe Ruge 1965" /> In their seminal 1965 study,<ref name="Galtung Holmboe Ruge 1965" /> Galtung and Ruge put forward a system of twelve factors describing events that together are used as defining "newsworthiness." Focusing on [[newspapers]] and [[Broadcast journalism|broadcast news]], Galtung and Ruge devised a list describing what they believed were significant contributing factors as to how the [[news]] is constructed. They proposed a "chain of news communication,"<ref name="Galtung Holmboe Ruge 1965" />{{rp|65}} which involves processes of selection (the more an event satisfies the "news factors," the more likely it is selected as news), distortion (accentuating the newsworthy factors of the event, once it has been selected), and replication (selection and distortion are repeated at all steps in the chain from event to reader). Furthermore, three basic hypotheses are presented by Galtung and Ruge: the additivity hypothesis that the more factors an event satisfies, the higher the probability that it becomes news; the complementary hypothesis that the factors will tend to exclude each other; and the exclusion hypothesis that events that satisfy none or very few factors will not become news. In 2001, the influential 1965 study was updated by Tony Harcup and Deirdre O'Neill, in a study of the [[British press]]. The findings of a content analysis of three major national newspapers in the UK were used to critically evaluate Galtung and Ruge's original criteria and to propose a contemporary set of news values. Forty years on, they found some notable differences, including the rise of [[celebrity]] news and that good news (as well as bad news) was a significant news value, as well as the newspaper's own agenda. They examined three [[tabloid newspapers]].<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Harcup | first1=Tony | last2=O'Neill | first2=Deirdre | date=2001 | title=What is news? : Galtung and Ruge revisited | journal=Journalism Studies | volume=2 | issue=2 | pages=261–280 | doi=10.1080/14616700118449 | issn=1461-670X | url=https://eprints.hud.ac.uk/id/eprint/27381/1/What%20is%20news%20Original.pdf}}</ref> == Contemporary news values == In a rapidly evolving market, achieving [[relevance]], giving [[audiences]] the news they want and find interesting, is an increasingly important goal for media outlets seeking to maintain market share. This has made news organizations more open to audience input and feedback, and forced them to adopt and apply news values that attract and keep audiences. Given these changes and the rapid rise of digital technology in recent years, Harcup and O’Neill updated their 2001 study in 2016,<ref>{{cite journal | doi=10.1080/1461670X.2016.1150193 | title=What is news? : News values revisited (again) | journal=Journalism Studies | volume=18 | issue=12 | pages=1470–1488 | year=2016 | last1=Harcup | first1=Tony | last2=O'Neill | first2=Deirdre | s2cid=147241691 | doi-access=free}}</ref> while other scholars have analysed news values in viral news shared via social media.<ref>{{cite journal | last=Bednarek | first=Monika | author-link1=Monika Bednarek | date=2016 | title=Investigating evaluation and news values in news items that are shared through social media | journal=Corpora | volume=11 | issue=2 | pages=227–257 | doi=10.3366/cor.2016.0093 | doi-access=free}}</ref> The growth of [[interactive media]] and [[citizen journalism]] is fast altering the traditional distinction between news producer and passive audience and may in future lead to a redefinition of what "news" means and the role of the news industry. == List of news values == A variety of external and internal pressures influence [[journalistic]] decisions during the [[news]]-making process, which can sometimes lead to [[Media bias|bias]] or unethical reporting. Many different factors have the potential to influence whether an event is first noticed by a [[news organisation]], second whether a [[news story|story]] will be written about that event, third, how that story is written, and fourth whether this story will end up being published as news and if so, where it is placed. Therefore, "there is no end to lists of news criteria."<ref>{{cite book | title=Prime time activism: Media strategies for grassroots organizing | url=https://archive.org/details/primetimeactivis0000ryan | url-access=registration | last=Ryan | first=Charlotte | publisher=South End Press | year=1991 | location=Boston | page=[https://archive.org/details/primetimeactivis0000ryan/page/31 31]}}</ref> There are multiple competing lists of news values (including Galtung & Ruge's news factors, and others put forward by Schlesinger,<ref name="Schlesinger 1987">{{cite book | last=Schlesinger | first=Philip | date=1987 | title=Putting 'reality' together: BBC News | edition=2nd | publication-place=London | publisher=Methuen | isbn=978-0-416-90190-0}} Originally published as {{cite book | last=Schlesinger | first=Philip | date=1978 | title=Putting 'reality' together: BBC News | publication-place=London | publisher=Constable | isbn=978-0-09-462040-7}} </ref> Bell,<ref name="Bell 1991">{{cite book | title=The language of news media | last=Bell | first=Allan | publisher=Blackwell | year=1991 | location=Oxford, UK | isbn=978-0-631-16434-0}}</ref> Bednarek & Caple<ref name="Bednarek Caple 2017" />), with considerable overlap but also disagreement as to what should be included.<ref name="Caple Bednarek 2013">{{cite web | last1=Caple | first1=Helen | last2=Bednarek | first2=Monika | date=December 2013 | title=Delving into the discourse: Approaches to news values in journalism studies and beyond | publication-place=Oxford, UK | publisher=Reuters Institute for the Study of Journalism, University of Oxford | url=https://ora.ox.ac.uk/objects/uuid:1f5c6d91-bb1f-4278-a160-66149ecfb36b/files/mc713432d59c70fe493d148ee29b73805 <!-- https://reutersinstitute.politics.ox.ac.uk/sites/default/files/2018-01/Delving%20into%20the%20Discourse.pdf -->}}</ref> News values can relate to aspects of events and actors, or to aspects of news gathering and processing:<ref name="Bell 1991" /> ''Values in news actors and events:'' *'''Frequency''': Events that occur suddenly and fit well with the news organization's schedule are more likely to be reported than those that occur gradually or at inconvenient times of day or night. Long-term trends are not likely to receive much coverage. *'''Timeliness''': Events that have only just happened, are current, ongoing, or are about to happen are newsworthy.<ref name="Bednarek Caple 2017" /> *'''Familiarity''': To do with people or places close to the [[target audience]]. Others prefer the term '''[[wikt:Special:Search/proximity|Proximity]]''' for this news value, which includes geographical and cultural proximity (see "meaningfulness").<ref name="Bednarek Caple 2017" /> *'''Negativity''': Bad news is more newsworthy than good news. Sometimes described as "the basic news value."<ref name="Bell 1991" /> Conversely, it has also been suggested that '''Positivity''' is a news value in certain cases (such as [[Sports journalism|sports news]], [[Science journalism|science news]], feel-good [[Tabloid journalism|tabloid]] stories). *'''Conflict''': Opposition of people or forces resulting in a dramatic effect. Events with conflict are often quite newsworthy. Sometimes included in Negativity rather than listed as a separate news value.<ref name="Bednarek Caple 2017">{{cite book | title=The discourse of news values: How news organizations create newsworthiness | last1=Bednarek | first1=Monika | author-link1=Monika Bednarek | last2=Caple | first2=Helen | publisher=Oxford University Press | year=2017 | isbn=978-0-19-065393-4 | location=Oxford, UK | doi=10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190653934.001.0001}}</ref> *'''Unexpectedness''': Events that are out of the ordinary, unexpected, or rare are more newsworthy than routine, unsurprising events.<ref name="Bell 1991" /><ref name="Bednarek Caple 2017" /> *'''Unambiguity''': Events whose implications are clear make for better copy than those that are open to more than one interpretation, or where any understanding of the implications depends on first understanding the complex background in which the events take place.<ref name="Bell 1991" /> *'''Personalization''': Events that can be portrayed as the actions of individuals will be more attractive than one in which there is no such "human interest." Personalization is about whether an event can be contextualised in personal terms (affecting or involving specific, "ordinary" people, not the generalised masses). *'''Meaningfulness''': This relates to the sense of identification the [[audience]] has with the topic. "Cultural proximity" is a factor here—events concerned with people who speak the same language, look the same, and share the same preoccupations as the audience receive more coverage than those concerned with people who speak different languages, look different and have different preoccupations. A related term is '''[[Relevance]]''', which is about the relevance of the event as regards the target readers/viewers own lives or how close it is to their experiences. '''Impact''' refers more generally to an event's impact, on the target audience, or on others. An event with significant consequences (high impact) is newsworthy.<ref name="Bednarek Caple 2017" /> *'''Eliteness''': Events concerned with [[Global Power|global powers]] receive more attention than those concerned with less influential nations. Events concerned with the rich, powerful, famous and infamous get more coverage. Also includes the eliteness of sources – sometimes called '''Attribution'''.<ref name="Bell 1991" /> *'''Superlativeness''': Events with a large scale or scope or with high intensity are newsworthy.<ref name="Bell 1991" /><ref name="Bednarek Caple 2017" /> *'''Consonance''': Events that fit with the media's expectations and preconceptions receive more coverage than those that defy them (and for which they are thus unprepared). Note this appears to conflict with unexpectedness above. However, consonance really refers to the media's ''readiness'' to report an item. Consonance has also been defined as relating to editors' stereotypes and their mental scripts for how events typically proceed.<ref name="Bell 1991" /> ''Values in the news process:'' *'''Continuity''': A story that is already in the news gathers a kind of inertia. This is partly because the media organizations are already in place to report the story, and partly because previous reportage may have made the story more accessible to the public (making it less ambiguous). *'''Composition''': Stories must compete with one another for space in the media. For instance, editors may seek to provide a balance of different types of coverage, so that if there is an excess of foreign news for instance, the least important foreign story may have to make way for an item concerned with the domestic news. In this way the prominence given to a story depends not only on its own news values but also on those of competing stories.<ref name="Galtung Holmboe Ruge 1965" /> *'''Competition''': Commercial or professional competition between media may lead [[journalists]] to endorse the news value given to a story by a rival. *'''Co-option''': A story that is only marginally newsworthy in its own right may be covered if it is related to a major running story.<ref name="Bell 1991" /> *'''Prefabrication''': A story that is marginal in news terms but written and available may be selected ahead of a much more newsworthy story that must be researched and written from the ground up.<ref name="Bell 1991" /> *'''Predictability''': An event is more likely to be covered if it has been pre-scheduled.<ref name="Bell 1991" /> *'''Story impact''': The impact of a published story (not the event), for example whether it is being shared widely (sometimes called '''Shareability'''), read, liked, commented-on. To be qualified as shareable, a story arguably has to be simple, emotional, unexpected and triggered. Engaging with such analytics is now an important part of newsroom practice. *'''Time constraints''': Traditional news media such as [[radio]], [[television]] and [[daily newspaper]]s have strict deadlines and a short production cycle, which selects for items that can be researched and covered quickly. *'''Logistics''': Although eased by the availability of [[global communication]]s even from remote regions, the ability to deploy and control production and [[Correspondent|reporting staff]], and functionality of technical resources can determine whether a story is covered.<ref name="Schlesinger 1987" /> *'''Data''': Media need to back up all of their stories with data in order to remain relevant and reliable. Reporters prefer to look at raw data in order to be able to take an unbiased perspective. An alternative term is '''Facticity''' – the favouring of facts and figures in hard news.<ref name="Bell 1991" /> One of the key differences in relation to these news values is whether they relate to events or stories. For example, composition and co-option both relate to the published news story. These are news values that concern how news stories fit with the other stories around them. The aim here is to ensure a balanced spread of stories with minimal duplication across a [[news program]] or edition.<ref>{{cite book | title=News values | url=https://archive.org/details/newsvalues00brig | url-access=limited | last1=Brighton | first1=Paul | last2=Foy | first2=Dennis | publisher=Sage | year=2007 | location=London | pages=[https://archive.org/details/newsvalues00brig/page/n36 26]}}</ref> Such news values are qualitatively different from news values that relate to aspects of events, such as Eliteness (the elite status of news actors or sources) or Proximity (the closeness of the event's location to the target audience). == Audience perceptions of news == Conventional models concentrate on what the journalist perceives as news. But the news process is a two-way transaction, involving both news producer (the journalist) and the news receiver (the audience), although the boundary between the two is rapidly blurring with the growth of citizen journalism and interactive media. Little has been done to define equivalent factors that determine audience perception of news. This is largely because it would appear impossible to define a common factor, or factors, that generate interest in a [[mass audience]]. Basing his judgement on many years as a newspaper journalist Hetherington states that: "...anything which threatens people's peace, prosperity and well being is news and likely to make headlines."<ref>{{cite book | last=Hetherington | first=Alastair | date=1985-10-25 | title=News, newspapers and television | publication-place=Basingstoke, UK | publisher=Macmillan | isbn=978-0-333-38606-4 | url=https://books.google.com/books?id=qKSwCwAAQBAJ&pg=PA40 | doi=10.1007/978-1-349-18000-4 | page=40}}</ref> Whyte-Venables suggests audiences may interpret news as a risk signal.<ref>{{cite book | last=Whyte-Venables | first=John | date=4 July 2012 | title=What is news? | edition=3rd | publisher=Willow Publishing | asin=B008HOADC6}} Revised from {{cite book | last=Venables | first=John | title=What is news? | publisher=Elm Publications | publication-place=Kings Ripton, UK | date=1993 | isbn=978-1-85450-052-6 | oclc=30437924}}</ref> Psychologists and [[primatologists]] have shown that apes and humans constantly monitor the environment for information that may signal the possibility of physical danger or threat to the individual's social position. This receptiveness to risk signals is a powerful and virtually universal survival mechanism. A "risk signal" is characterized by two factors, an element of change (or uncertainty) and the relevance of that change to the security of the individual. The same two conditions are observed to be characteristic of news. The news value of a story, if defined in terms of the interest it carries for an audience, is determined by the degree of change it contains and the relevance that change has for the individual or group. Analysis shows that journalists and publicists manipulate both the element of change and relevance ('security concern') to maximize, or some cases play down, the strength of a story. Security concern is proportional to the relevance of the story for the individual, his or her family, social group and societal group, in declining order. At some point there is a Boundary of Relevance, beyond which the change is no longer perceived to be relevant, or newsworthy. This boundary may be manipulated by journalists, power elites and communicators seeking to encourage audiences to exclude, or embrace, certain groups: for instance, to distance a home audience from the enemy in time of [[war]], or conversely, to highlight the plight of a distant culture so as to encourage support for aid programs.<ref>{{cite book | title=Source journalism and news values | last=Landau | first=Joel | year=2016 | isbn=978-1-365-44689-4 | ol=OL17372971W | page=1 | language=en}} {{sps|certain=yes|date=May 2024}}</ref>{{better source needed|date=May 2024}} In 2018, Hal Pashler and Gail Heriot published a study showing that perceptions of newsworthiness tend to be contaminated by a political usefulness bias. In other words, individuals tend to view stories that give them "ammunition" for their political views as more newsworthy. They give credence to their own views.<ref>{{cite journal | last1=Pashler | first1=Harold | last2=Heriot | first2=Gail | date=2018 | title=Perceptions of newsworthiness are contaminated by a political usefulness bias | journal=Royal Society Open Science | volume=5 | issue=8 | page=172239 | issn=2054-5703 | pmid=30224994 | pmc=6124072 | doi=10.1098/rsos.172239 | doi-access=free | bibcode=2018RSOS....572239P | ssrn=3225878}}</ref> == Evolutionary perspectives == An [[evolutionary psychology]] explanation for why negative news have a higher news value than positive news starts with the empirical observation that the human perceptive system and lower level brain functions have difficulty distinguishing between media stimuli and real stimuli. These lower level brain mechanisms which function on a subconscious level make basic evaluations of perceptive stimuli, focus attention on important stimuli, and start basic emotional reactions. Research has also found that the brain differentiates between negative and positive stimuli and reacts quicker and more automatically to negative stimuli which are also better remembered. This likely has evolutionary explanations with it often being important to quickly focus attention on, evaluate, and quickly respond to threats. While the reaction to a strong negative stimulus is to avoid, a moderately negative stimulus instead causes curiosity and further examination. Negative media news is argued to fall into the latter category which explains their popularity. Lifelike audiovisual media are argued to have particularly strong effects compared to reading.<ref name="Grabe 2011">{{cite book | last1=Grabe | first1=Maria Elizabeth | chapter=News as reality-inducing, survival-relevant, and gender-specific stimuli | pages=361–377 | title=Applied evolutionary psychology | date=24 November 2011 | publisher=Oxford University Press | isbn=978-0-19-958607-3 | doi=10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.003.0022 | editor1-last=Roberts | editor1-first=S. Craig}}</ref> Women have on average stronger avoidance reactions to moderately negative stimuli. Men and women also differ on average in how they enjoy, evaluate, remember, comprehend, and identify with the people in news depending on if the news are negatively or positively framed. The stronger avoidance reaction to moderately negative stimuli has been explained as it being the role of men in evolutionary history to investigate and potentially respond aggressively to threats while women and children withdrew. It has been claimed that negative news are framed according to male preferences by the often male journalists who cover such news and that a more positive framing may attract a larger female audience.<ref name="Grabe 2011" /> However, other scholars have urged caution as regards evolutionary psychology's claims about gender differences.<ref>{{cite journal | last=Cameron | first=Deborah | date=2010 | title=Sex/gender, language and the new biologism | journal=Applied Linguistics | volume=31 | issue=2 | pages=173–192 | doi=10.1093/applin/amp022}}</ref> == See also == {{div col|colwidth=30em}} * [[Afghanistanism]] * [[Agenda-setting theory]] * [[News bias]] * [[Mass media impact on spatial perception]] * [[Media imperialism]] * [[Media transparency]] * [[Reporting bias]] * [[Systemic bias]] * [[The Media Equation]] {{div col end}} == Notes == {{reflist|colwidth=30em}} == References == * {{cite book | last=Gans | first=Herbert J. | year=2004 | orig-year=1979 | title=Deciding what's news: A study of ''CBS Evening News'', ''NBC Nightly News'', ''Newsweek'', and ''Time'' | edition=25th anniversary | publication-place=Evanston, Illinois | publisher=Northwestern University Press | series=Medill School of Journalism visions of the American press | isbn=978-0-8101-2237-6 | url=https://books.google.com/books?id=bWpFtVJlAD0C}} * {{cite journal | last=Östgaard | first=Einar | date=1965 | title=Factors influencing the flow of news | journal=Journal of Peace Research | volume=2 | issue=1 | pages=39–63 | doi=10.1177/002234336500200103 | issn=0022-3433 | jstor=423010 | url=https://globalvoices.org/wp-content/uploads/2008/11/39.pdf}} * {{cite journal | last=Schulz | first=Winfried Friedrich | date=1982 | title=News structure and people's awareness of political events | journal=Gazette (Leiden, Netherlands) | volume=30 | issue=3 | doi=10.1177/001654928203000301 | issn=0016-5492 | pages=139–153}} == External links == * [https://www.facebook.com/vanessa.otero.9619/posts/10155006385626062 Chart – Real and Fake News (2016)/Vanessa Otero] ([https://web.archive.org/web/20170708003907/http://www.allgeneralizationsarefalse.com/?p=65 basis]) ([https://boingboing.net/2016/12/14/an-attempt-to-chart-new-source.html Mark Frauenfelder]) * [http://www.journalism.org/2014/10/21/political-polarization-media-habits/pj_14-10-21_mediapolarization-08/ Chart – Real and Fake News (2014)] ([http://www.pewresearch.org/pj_14-10-21_mediapolarization-08-2/ 2016])/[[Pew Research Center]] * [http://www.newsvaluesanalysis.com/ Discursive News Values Analysis] {{Journalism}} {{Media and human factors}} {{portal bar|Journalism}} [[Category:Journalism standards]] [[Category:Business culture]] [[Category:Mass media events]]
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Pages transcluded onto the current version of this page
(
help
)
:
Template:Better source needed
(
edit
)
Template:Cite book
(
edit
)
Template:Cite journal
(
edit
)
Template:Cite news
(
edit
)
Template:Cite web
(
edit
)
Template:Div col
(
edit
)
Template:Div col end
(
edit
)
Template:Journalism
(
edit
)
Template:Journalism sidebar
(
edit
)
Template:Media and human factors
(
edit
)
Template:Portal bar
(
edit
)
Template:Redirect
(
edit
)
Template:Reflist
(
edit
)
Template:Rp
(
edit
)
Template:Short description
(
edit
)
Template:Sps
(
edit
)
Template:Use dmy dates
(
edit
)