Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Observer-expectancy effect
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
{{Short description|Cognitive bias of experimental subject}} {{Other uses|Observer effect (disambiguation)|Expectancy effect (disambiguation){{!}}Expectancy effect}} {{Multiple| {{More citations needed|date=October 2021}} {{Content|On_the_revision_940218724|date=February 2020}} }} {{Psychology sidebar}} The '''observer-expectancy effect'''{{efn|Also called the '''experimenter-expectancy effect''', '''expectancy bias''', '''observer effect''', or '''experimenter effect'''.}} is a form of [[Reactivity (psychology)|reactivity]] in which a [[researcher]]'s [[cognitive bias]] causes them to subconsciously influence the participants of an experiment. [[Confirmation bias]] can lead to the [[experiment]]er interpreting results incorrectly because of the tendency to look for information that conforms to their hypothesis, and overlook information that argues against it.<ref>Goldstein, Bruce. "Cognitive Psychology". Wadsworth, Cengage Learning, 2011, p. 374</ref> It is a significant threat to a study's [[internal validity]], and is therefore typically [[experimental control|controlled]] using a [[double-blind experiment|double-blind experimental design]]. It may include conscious or unconscious influences on subject behavior including creation of [[demand characteristics]] that influence subjects, and altered or selective recording of experimental results themselves.<ref name="KantowitzIII2009">{{cite book|author1=Barry H. Kantowitz|author2=Henry L. Roediger, III|author3=David G. Elmes|title=Experimental Psychology|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=2-5VL8PHLsIC&pg=PA371|access-date=7 September 2013|year=2009|publisher=Cengage Learning|isbn=978-0-495-59533-5|page=371}}</ref> == Overview == The experimenter may introduce cognitive bias into a study in several {{nowrap|ways{{px2}}{{mdash}}{{px2}}}}in the observer-expectancy effect, the experimenter may subtly communicate their expectations for the outcome of the study to the participants, causing them to alter their behavior to conform to those expectations. Such observer bias effects are near-universal in human data interpretation under expectation and in the presence of imperfect cultural and methodological norms that promote or enforce objectivity.<ref>{{cite book |first=R. |last=Rosenthal |title=Experimenter Effects in Behavioral Research |location=NY |publisher=Appleton-Century-Crofts |year=1966}}</ref> The classic example of experimenter bias is that of "[[Clever Hans]]", an [[Orlov Trotter]] [[horse]] claimed by his owner von Osten to be able to do [[Elementary arithmetic|arithmetic]] and other tasks. As a result of the large public interest in Clever Hans, [[philosopher]] and [[psychologist]] [[Carl Stumpf]], along with his assistant [[Oskar Pfungst]], investigated these claims. Ruling out simple fraud, Pfungst determined that the horse could answer correctly even when von Osten did not ask the questions. However, the horse was unable to answer correctly when either it could not see the questioner, or if the questioner themselves was unaware of the correct answer: When von Osten knew the answers to the questions, Hans answered correctly 89% of the time. However, when von Osten did not know the answers, Hans guessed only 6% of questions correctly. Pfungst then proceeded to examine the behaviour of the questioner in detail, and showed that as the horse's taps approached the right answer, the questioner's [[Human position|posture]] and facial expression changed in ways that were consistent with an increase in tension, which was released when the horse made the final, correct tap. This provided a cue that the horse had learned to use as a [[Reinforcement learning|reinforced]] cue to stop tapping.{{Citation needed|date=October 2023}} Experimenter-bias also influences human subjects. As an example, researchers compared performance of two groups given the same task (rating portrait pictures and estimating how successful each individual was on a scale of β10 to 10), but with different experimenter expectations.{{Citation needed|date=October 2023}} In one group, ("Group A"), experimenters were told to expect positive ratings while in another group, ("Group B"), experimenters were told to expect negative ratings. Data collected from Group A was a significant and substantially more optimistic appraisal than the data collected from Group B. The researchers suggested that experimenters gave subtle but clear cues with which the subjects [[Asch conformity experiments|complied]].<ref>Rosenthal R. ''Experimenter Effects in Behavioral Research''. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1966. 464 p.</ref> ==Prevention== {{unreferenced section|date=July 2024}} [[Double blind]] techniques may be employed to combat bias by causing the experimenter and subject to be ignorant of which condition data flows from. It might be thought that, due to the [[central limit theorem]] of statistics, collecting more independent measurements will improve the precision of estimates, thus decreasing bias. However, this assumes that the measurements are statistically independent. In the case of experimenter bias, the measures share correlated bias: simply averaging such data will not lead to a better statistic but may merely reflect the correlations among the individual measurements and their non-independent nature. == Notes == {{notelist}} ==See also== {{stack|{{Portal|Society|Psychology}} }} {{div col|colwidth=30em}} * [[List of cognitive biases]] * [[Allegiance bias]] * [[Cultural bias]] * [[Demand characteristics]] * [[Epistemic feedback]] * [[Funding bias]] * [[Hawthorne effect]] * [[N ray]]s β imaginary radiation * [[Naturalistic observation]] * [[Observer bias]] * [[Participant observer]] * [[Placebo]] and [[Nocebo]] * [[Publication bias]] * [[Pygmalion effect]] β teachers who expect higher achievement from some children actually get it * [[Reality tunnel]] * [[Reflexivity (social theory)]] * [[Subject-expectancy effect]] * [[Systematic bias]] * [[White hat bias|White-hat bias]]{{div col end}} ==References== {{reflist|2}} ==External links== * [http://skepdic.com/experimentereffect.html Skeptic's Dictionary on the Experimenter Effect] {{Biases}} {{Fallacies}} {{Hidden messages}} {{DEFAULTSORT:Observer-Expectancy Effect}} [[Category:Design of experiments]] [[Category:Cognitive inertia]]
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Pages transcluded onto the current version of this page
(
help
)
:
Template:Biases
(
edit
)
Template:Citation needed
(
edit
)
Template:Cite book
(
edit
)
Template:Div col
(
edit
)
Template:Div col end
(
edit
)
Template:Efn
(
edit
)
Template:Fallacies
(
edit
)
Template:Hidden messages
(
edit
)
Template:Multiple
(
edit
)
Template:Notelist
(
edit
)
Template:Nowrap
(
edit
)
Template:Other uses
(
edit
)
Template:Psychology sidebar
(
edit
)
Template:Reflist
(
edit
)
Template:Short description
(
edit
)
Template:Sidebar with collapsible lists
(
edit
)
Template:Stack
(
edit
)
Template:Unreferenced section
(
edit
)