Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Phrase structure rules
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
{{Short description|Rewrite rule used to describe a given language's syntax}} '''Phrase structure rules''' are a type of [[rewrite rule]] used to describe a given language's [[syntax]] and are closely associated with the early stages of [[transformational grammar]], proposed by [[Noam Chomsky]] in 1957.<ref>For general discussions of phrase structure rules, see for instance Borsley (1991:34ff.), Brinton (2000:165), Falk (2001:46ff.).</ref> They are used to break down a natural [[language]] sentence into its constituent parts, also known as [[syntactic category|syntactic categories]], including both lexical categories ([[part of speech|parts of speech]]) and [[phrase|phrasal]] categories. A grammar that uses phrase structure rules is a type of [[phrase structure grammar]]<!-- [needs clarification or removal:] - except in [[computer science]], where it is known as just a [[Formal grammar|grammar]], usually [[context-free grammar|context-free]]-->. Phrase structure rules as they are commonly employed operate according to the [[constituent (linguistics)|constituency]] relation, and a grammar that employs phrase structure rules is therefore a [[phrase structure grammar|''constituency grammar'']]; as such, it stands in contrast to [[dependency grammar|''dependency grammars'']], which are based on the [[government (linguistics)|dependency]] relation.<ref>Dependency grammars are associated above all with the work of Lucien Tesnière (1959).</ref> ==Definition and examples== Phrase structure rules are usually of the following form: :<math>A \to B \quad C</math> meaning that the [[constituent (linguistics)|constituent]] <math>A</math> is separated into the two subconstituents <math>B</math> and <math>C</math>. Some examples for English are as follows: :<chem>S -> NP \quad VP</chem> :<chem>NP -> (Det) \quad N1</chem> :<chem>N1 -> (AP) \quad N1 \quad (PP)</chem> The first rule reads: A S ([[Sentence (linguistics)|sentence]]) consists of a NP ([[noun phrase]]) followed by a VP ([[verb phrase]]). The second rule reads: A noun phrase consists of an optional Det ([[determiner]]) followed by a N (noun). The third rule means that a N (noun) can be preceded by an optional AP ([[adjective phrase]]) and followed by an optional PP ([[prepositional phrase]]). The round brackets indicate optional constituents. Beginning with the sentence symbol S, and applying the phrase structure rules successively, finally applying replacement rules to substitute actual words for the abstract symbols, it is possible to generate many proper sentences of English (or whichever language the rules are specified for). If the rules are correct, then any sentence produced in this way ought to be grammatically (syntactically) [[grammaticality|correct]]. It is also to be expected that the rules will generate syntactically correct but [[semantics|semantically]] nonsensical sentences, such as the following well-known example: ::[[Colorless green ideas sleep furiously]] This sentence was constructed by [[Noam Chomsky]] as an illustration that phrase structure rules are capable of generating syntactically correct but semantically incorrect sentences. Phrase structure rules break sentences down into their constituent parts. These constituents are often represented as [[parse tree|tree structures]] ([[dendrogram]]s). The tree for Chomsky's sentence can be rendered as follows: :[[image:cgisf-tgg.svg|300px|Colorless green ideas sleep furiously.]] A constituent is any word or combination of words that is dominated by a single node. Thus each individual word is a constituent. Further, the subject NP ''Colorless green ideas'', the minor NP ''green ideas'', and the VP ''sleep furiously'' are constituents. Phrase structure rules and the tree structures that are associated with them are a form of [[immediate constituent analysis]]. In [[transformational grammar]], systems of phrase structure rules are supplemented by transformation rules, which act on an existing syntactic structure to produce a new one (performing such operations as [[negation (linguistics)|negation]], [[passivization]], etc.). These transformations are not strictly required for generation, as the sentences they produce could be generated by a suitably expanded system of phrase structure rules alone, but transformations provide greater economy and enable significant relations between sentences to be reflected in the grammar. ==Top down== An important aspect of phrase structure rules is that they view sentence structure from the top down. The category on the left of the arrow is a greater constituent and the immediate constituents to the right of the arrow are lesser constituents. Constituents are successively broken down into their parts as one moves down a list of phrase structure rules for a given sentence. This top-down view of sentence structure stands in contrast to much work done in modern theoretical syntax. In [[Minimalist program|Minimalism]]<ref>See for instance Chomsky (1995).</ref> for instance, sentence structure is generated from the bottom up. The operation [[Merge (linguistics)|Merge]] merges smaller constituents to create greater constituents until the greatest constituent (i.e. the sentence) is reached. In this regard, theoretical syntax abandoned phrase structure rules long ago, although their importance for [[computational linguistics]] seems to remain intact. ==Alternative approaches== ===Constituency vs. dependency=== Phrase structure rules as they are commonly employed result in a view of sentence structure that is ''constituency-based''. Thus, grammars that employ phrase structure rules are ''constituency grammars'' (= [[phrase structure grammar]]s), as opposed to ''[[dependency grammar]]s'',<ref>The most comprehensive source on dependency grammar is Ágel et al. (2003/6).</ref> which view sentence structure as ''dependency-based''. What this means is that for phrase structure rules to be applicable at all, one has to pursue a constituency-based understanding of sentence structure. The constituency relation is a one-to-one-or-more correspondence. For every word in a sentence, there is at least one node in the syntactic structure that corresponds to that word. The dependency relation, in contrast, is a one-to-one relation; for every word in the sentence, there is exactly one node in the syntactic structure that corresponds to that word. The distinction is illustrated with the following trees: :[[File:Phrase structure rules.jpg|Phrase structure rules: Constituency vs. dependency]] The constituency tree on the left could be generated by phrase structure rules. The sentence S is broken down into smaller and smaller constituent parts. The dependency tree on the right could not, in contrast, be generated by phrase structure rules (at least not as they are commonly interpreted). ===Representational grammars=== A number of representational phrase structure theories of grammar never acknowledged phrase structure rules, but have pursued instead an understanding of sentence structure in terms the notion of [[Schema (psychology)|schema]]. Here phrase structures are not derived from rules that combine words, but from the specification or instantiation of syntactic schemata or configurations, often expressing some kind of semantic content independently of the specific words that appear in them. This approach is essentially equivalent to a system of phrase structure rules combined with a non[[compositionality|compositional]] [[semantics|semantic]] theory, since grammatical formalisms based on rewriting rules are generally equivalent in power to those based on substitution into schemata. So in this type of approach, instead of being derived from the application of a number of phrase structure rules, the sentence ''Colorless green ideas sleep furiously'' would be generated by filling the words into the slots of a schema having the following structure: ::[NP[ADJ N] VP[V] AP<nowiki>[ADV]]</nowiki> And which would express the following conceptual content: ::X DOES Y IN THE MANNER OF Z Though they are non-compositional, such models are monotonic. This approach is highly developed within [[Construction grammar]]<ref>Concerning Construction Grammar, see Goldberg (2006).</ref> and has had some influence in [[Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar]]<ref>Concerning Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar, see Pollard and Sag (1994).</ref> and [[lexical functional grammar]],<ref>Concerning Lexical Functional Grammar, see Bresnan (2001).</ref> the latter two clearly qualifying as phrase structure grammars. ==See also== {{cmn| *[[Constituent (linguistics)|Constituent]] *[[Dependency grammar]] *[[Discontinuous-constituent phrase structure grammar]] *[[Immediate constituent analysis]] *[[Non-configurational language]] *[[Phrase]] *[[Phrase structure grammar]] *[[Syntactic category]] }} ==Notes== {{Reflist}} ==References== {{refbegin|2}} *Ágel, V., Ludwig Eichinger, Hans-Werner Eroms, Peter Hellwig, Hans Heringer, and Hennig Lobin (eds.) 2003/6. [https://books.google.com/books?id=GWhp8IJ20X4C&q=%22Phrase+structure+rules%22 Dependency and Valency: An International Handbook of Contemporary Research]. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. *Borsley, R. 1991. Syntactic theory: A unified approach. London: Edward Arnold. *Bresnan, Joan 2001. Lexical Functional Syntax. *Brinton, L. 2000. [https://books.google.com/books?id=9MO7Mlg2RnMC The structure of modern English]. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. *Carnie, A. 2013. [https://books.google.com/books?id=or-Y3c9dY4UC Syntax: A Generative Introduction], 3rd edition. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing. *Chomsky, N. 1957. Syntactic Structures. The Hague/Paris: Mouton. *Chomsky, N. 1995. The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press. *Falk, Y. 2001. Lexical-Functional Grammar: An introduction to parallel constraint-based syntax. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications. *Goldberg, A. 2006. [https://books.google.com/books?id=n1rsCecF-_4C Constructions at Work: The Nature of Generalization in Language]. Oxford University Press. *Pollard, C. and I. Sag 1994. [https://books.google.com/books?id=Ftvg8Vo3QHwC Head-driven phrase structure grammar]. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. *Tesnière, L. 1959. Éleménts de syntaxe structurale. Paris: Klincksieck. {{refend}} [[Category:Grammar frameworks]] [[Category:Generative syntax]] [[Category:Syntactic theories]] [[Category:Phrases|structure rules]] [[Category:Syntax]] [[Category:Syntactic categories]] [[Category:Syntactic relationships]]
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Pages transcluded onto the current version of this page
(
help
)
:
Template:Cmn
(
edit
)
Template:Refbegin
(
edit
)
Template:Refend
(
edit
)
Template:Reflist
(
edit
)
Template:Short description
(
edit
)