Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Self-serving bias
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
{{short description|Distortion to enhance self-esteem, or to see oneself overly favorably}} A '''self-serving bias''' is any cognitive or perceptual process that is distorted by the need to maintain and enhance [[self-esteem]], or the tendency to perceive oneself in an overly favorable manner.<ref>Myers, D.G. (2015). ''Exploring Social Psychology'', 7th Edition. New York: McGraw Hill Education.</ref> It is the belief that individuals tend to ascribe success to their own abilities and efforts, but ascribe failure to external factors.<ref name="Campbell & Sedikides"/> When individuals reject the validity of negative feedback, focus on their strengths and achievements but overlook their faults and failures, or take more credit for their group's work than they give to other members, they are protecting their self-esteem from threat and injury. These cognitive and perceptual tendencies perpetuate illusions and error, but they also serve the self's need for esteem.<ref>{{cite encyclopedia|author-link1=Donelson R. Forsyth|last=Forsyth|first=Donelson|entry=Self-Serving Bias|url=https://facultystaff.richmond.edu/~dforsyth/pubs/forsyth2008selfserving.pdf|title=International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences|isbn=9780028661179|edition=2nd|publisher=Macmillan Reference USA|date=2007}}</ref> For example, a student who [[Attribution (psychology)|attributes]] earning a good grade on an exam to their own intelligence and preparation but attributes earning a poor grade to the teacher's poor teaching ability or unfair test questions might be exhibiting a self-serving bias. Studies have shown that similar attributions are made in various situations, such as the workplace,<ref name="Pal">{{cite journal|last=Pal|first=G.C.|year=2007|title=Is there a universal self-serving attribution bias?|url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/311450773|journal=Psychological Studies|volume=52|issue=1|pages=85β89}}</ref> [[interpersonal relationships]],<ref name="Campbell et al.">{{cite journal|last=Campbell|first=W. Keith |author2=Sedikides, Constantine |author3=Reeder, Glenn D. |author4=Elliot, Andrew J.|title=Among friends? An examination of friendship and the self-serving bias|journal=British Journal of Social Psychology|year=2000|volume=39|issue=2|pages=229β239|doi=10.1348/014466600164444|pmid=10907097 |citeseerx=10.1.1.559.7984 }}</ref> sports,<ref name="DeMichele">{{cite journal|last=De Michele|first=P.|author2=Gansneder, B.|author3=Solomon, G.|year=1998|title=Success and failure attributions of wrestlers: Further Evidence of the Self-Serving Bias|url=http://psycnet.apa.org/record/1998-10476-002|journal=Journal of Sport Behavior|volume=21|issue=3|pages=242}}</ref> and consumer decisions.<ref name=Moon>{{cite journal|last=Moon|first=Youngme|title=Don't Blame the Computer: When Self-Disclosure Moderates the Self-Serving Bias|journal=Journal of Consumer Psychology|year=2003|volume=13|issue=1|pages=125β137|doi=10.1207/153276603768344843}}</ref> Both motivational processes (i.e. [[self-enhancement]], [[self-presentation]]) and cognitive processes (i.e. [[locus of control]], self-esteem) influence the self-serving bias.<ref name=Shepperd>{{cite journal|last=Shepperd|first=James |author2=Malone, Wendi |author3=Sweeny, Kate|title=Exploring Causes of the Self-serving Bias|journal=Social and Personality Psychology Compass|year=2008|volume=2|issue=2|pages=895β908|doi=10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00078.x|s2cid=51959777 }}</ref> There are both cross-cultural (i.e. [[individualistic]] and [[Collectivism and individualism|collectivistic]] culture differences) and special clinical population (i.e. [[Depression (mood)|depression]]) considerations within the bias.<ref name=Hooghiemstra>{{cite journal|last=Hooghiemstra|first=Reggy|title=East-West Differences in Attributions for Company Performance: A Content Analysis of Japanese and U.S. Corporate Annual Reports|journal=Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology|year=2008|volume=39|issue=5|pages=618β629|doi=10.1177/0022022108321309|s2cid=145404974}}</ref><ref name="Greenberg et al">{{cite journal|last=Greenberg|first=Jeff |author2=Pyszczynski, Tom |author3=Burling, John |author4=Tibbs, Karyn|title=Depression, self-focused attention, and the self-serving attributional bias|journal=Personality and Individual Differences|year=1992|volume=13|issue=9|pages=959β965|doi=10.1016/0191-8869(92)90129-D}}</ref> Much of the research on the self-serving bias has used participant self-reports of attribution based on experimental manipulation of task outcomes or in naturalistic situations.<ref name="Campbell & Sedikides" /> Some more modern research, however, has shifted focus to physiological manipulations, such as emotional inducement and neural activation, in an attempt to better understand the biological mechanisms that contribute to the self-serving bias.<ref name="Krusemark et al">{{cite journal|title=Attributions, deception, and event related potentials: An investigation of the self-serving bias|journal=Psychophysiology|year=2008|volume=45|issue=4|pages=511β515|doi=10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00659.x|pmid=18282197|last1=Krusemark|first1=E. A.|last2= Campbell|first2=W. Keith|last3=Clementz|first3=B. A.}}</ref><ref name="Blackwood-2003" /> ==History== The theory of self-serving biases first came to attention in the late 1960s to early 1970s. As research on this topic grew, some people had concerns about it.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Miller|first=Dale|author2=Michael Ross|date=1975|title=Self-serving Biases in the Attribution of Causality: Fact or Fiction?|url=http://web.mit.edu/curhan/www/docs/Articles/biases/82_Psychological_Bulletin_213_(Miller).pdf|journal=Psychological Bulletin|volume=82|issue=2|pages=213β225|doi=10.1037/h0076486}}</ref> In 1971, a fear emerged that the hypothesis would prove to be incorrect, much like the perceptual defense hypothesis by Dixon. However, the theory now holds strong. When this theory was still being developed it was during the research of [[attribution bias]]. [[Fritz Heider]] found that in ambiguous situations people made attributions based on their own needs, in order to maintain a higher self-esteem and viewpoint. This specific tendency became what we now know as the self-serving bias. Miller and Ross conducted a study in 1975 that was one of the earliest to assess not only self-serving bias but also the attributions for successes and failures within this theory.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Larson|first=James|author2=Rutger U |author3=Douglass Coll |title=Evidence for a self-serving bias in the attribution of causality|journal=Journal of Personality|volume=45|issue=3|pages=430β441|doi=10.1111/j.1467-6494.1977.tb00162.x |year=1977}}</ref> They argued that the self-serving bias people create is rational and not dependent on one's need for self-esteem. This means that if the outcome of an event is consistent with the person's expectation, then they will attribute dispositional (internal) factors. On the other hand, if the outcome of the event does not match the person's expectations, they will make situational attributions by blaming their surroundings instead of themselves. ==Methods== ===Laboratory testing=== Investigations of the self-serving bias in the laboratory differ depending on the experimental goals, but have basic fundamental aspects. Participants perform some task, often of intelligence, social sensitivity, teaching ability, or therapy skills.<ref name="Campbell & Sedikides" /> Participants may be asked to work alone, in pairs, or in groups. After task completion, participants are given randomized bogus feedback. Some studies employ emotion-induction mechanisms to investigate moderating effects on the self-serving bias.<ref name=Coleman /> Finally, participants make attributions for the given outcomes. These attributions are assessed by the researchers to determine implications for the self-serving bias.<ref name="Campbell & Sedikides" /> ===Neural experimentation=== Some more modern testing employs neural imaging techniques to supplement the fundamental self-serving bias laboratory procedures. Neural correlates of the self-serving bias have been investigated by [[electroencephalography]] (EEG),<ref name="Blackwood-2003" /> as well as [[functional magnetic resonance imaging]] (fMRI).<ref name="Krusemark et al" /> These procedures allow for insight into brain area activity during exhibition of a self-serving bias, as well as a mechanism to differentiate brain activity between healthy and clinical populations.<ref name="Seidel-2011">{{cite journal|last=Seidel|first=Eva-Maria|author2=Satterthwaite, Theodore D. |author3=Eickhoff, Simon B. |author4=Schneider, Frank |author5=Gur, Ruben C. |author6=Wolf, Daniel H. |author7=Habel, Ute |author8=Derntl, Birgit |title=Neural correlates of depressive realism β An fMRI study on causal attribution in depression|journal=Journal of Affective Disorders|year=2011|volume=138|issue=3|pages=268β276|doi=10.1016/j.jad.2012.01.041|pmid=22377511 |pmc=3565123}}</ref> ===Naturalistic investigation=== Retrospective performance outcomes can be used in investigation of the self-serving bias. An example of this is reported company performance followed up by self-report of outcome attributions.<ref name=Hooghiemstra /> These self-report attributions can then be used to assess how successes and failures are viewed by company employees and executives. This method can be used for numerous outcome variables to determine the presence or absence of the self-serving bias. ==Factors and variables== ===Motivation=== Two types of [[motivation]] affect the self-serving bias: [[self-enhancement]] and [[self-presentation]].<ref name=Shepperd /> Self-enhancement aims to uphold self-worth; attributing successes internally and failures externally helps people in their self-enhancement. Self-presentation refers to the drive to convey a desired image to others and make self-serving attributions to manage impressions.<ref name=Shepperd /> For example, they claim personal responsibility for successes but not failures in an attempt to influence how others perceive them. Motivation works in conjunction with cognitive factors to produce personally satisfying and self-preserving attributions for outcomes.<ref name=Shepperd /> ===Locus of control=== [[Locus of control]] is one of the main influences of [[Attribution (psychology)|attribution]] style. Individuals with an internal locus of control believe that they have personal control over situations and that their actions matter. Those with an external locus of control believe that outside forces, chance, and luck determine situations and that their actions cannot change anything.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Twenge|first=Jean M. |author2=Zhang, Liqing |author3=Im, Charles|title=It's Beyond My Control: A Cross-Temporal Meta-Analysis of Increasing Externality in Locus of Control, 1960-2002|journal=Personality and Social Psychology Review|year=2004|volume=8|issue=3|pages=308β319|doi=10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_5|pmid=15454351 |citeseerx=10.1.1.532.5200 |s2cid=13318634 }}</ref> Individuals with an external locus of control are more likely to exhibit a self-serving bias following failure than those with an internal locus of control.<ref name="Campbell & Sedikides">{{cite journal|last=Campbell|first=W. Keith|author2=Sedikides, Constantine |title=Self-threat magnifies the self-serving bias: A meta-analytic integration.|journal=Review of General Psychology|year=1999|volume=3|issue=1|pages=23β43|doi=10.1037/1089-2680.3.1.23|s2cid=144756539}}</ref><ref name=Wichman>{{cite journal|last=Wichman|first=Harvey|author2=Ball, James |title=Locus of control, self-serving biases, and attitudes towards safety in general aviation pilots|journal=Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine|year=1983|volume=54|issue=6|pages=507β510|pmid=6882309}}</ref> The difference in attribution style between individuals with internal and external loci of control, however, is not as marked in successful outcomes, as individuals with both types attribution style have less need to defend their [[self-image]]s in success. Airplane pilots with an internal locus of control were likely to exhibit a self-serving bias in regard to their skill and levels of safety.<ref name=Wichman /> ===Gender=== Studies have shown a slight discrepancy in males' and females' use of the self-serving bias. In self-report surveys investigating partner interactions of romantic couples, men tended to attribute negative interactions to their partners more than women did.<ref name="Christensen">{{cite journal|last=Christensen|first=A.|author2=Sullaway, M.|author3=King, C. E.|year=1983|title=Systematic error in behavioral reports of dyadic interaction: Egocentric bias and content effects|url=http://psycnet.apa.org/record/1983-31566-001|journal=Behavioral Assessment|volume=5|issue=2|pages=129β140}}</ref> This is evidence that men may exhibit the self-serving bias more than women, although the study did not look at positive interaction attributions. ===Age=== Older adults have been shown to make more internal causal attributions for negative outcomes.<ref name=Lachman>{{cite journal|last=Lachman|first=M.|title=When Bad Things Happen to Older People: Age Differences in A ttributional Style|journal=Psychology and Aging|year=1990|volume=5|issue=4|pages=607β609|doi=10.1037/0882-7974.5.4.607|pmid=2278689}}</ref> Differential attribution style at different ages indicates that the self-serving bias may be less likely in older adults. These older adults who attributed negative outcomes to more internal factors also rated themselves to be in poorer health, so negative emotional factors may confound the found age effects. ===Culture=== There is evidence of cross-cultural differences in the tendency to exhibit the self-serving bias, particularly when considering individualistic (Western) versus collectivistic (non-Western) societies.<ref name=Al-Zah>{{cite journal|last=Al-Zahrini|first=S.|author2=Kaplowitz, S. |title=Attributional Biases in Individualistic and Collectivistic Cultures: A Comparison of Americans with Saudis|journal=Social Psychology Quarterly|year=1993|volume=56|issue=3|pages=223β233|doi=10.2307/2786780|jstor=2786780}}</ref> Family and group goals are important in collectivistic cultures. In contrast, the individual goals and identity focused on in individualistic societies increases the need for people within those cultures to guard and boost their personal self-esteem. While differences have been shown, conflicting literature has cited similarity in causal attributions across both individual and collective cultures, specifically between Belgium, West Germany, South Korea, and England.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Schuster|first=B.|author2=Forsterlung, F. |author3=Weiner, B. |title=Perceiving the Causes of Success and Failure: A Cross-Cultural Examination of Attributional Concepts|journal=Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology|year=1989|volume=20|issue=2|pages=191β213|doi=10.1177/0022022189202005|s2cid=145528480}}</ref> Naturalistic observation and information comparing United States and Japanese companies outcome attributions shows that the meaning and psychological function of internal versus external attributions are similar across cultures but that the difference is in the strategy of attribution.<ref name=Hooghiemstra/> No consensus has been reached on cross-culture influences on the self-serving bias, though some systematic differences do seem to be present, especially between Western and non-Western cultures. For example, a study conducted by Kudo and Numuzaki showed that the participants in the success condition provided more internal attributions than the participants in the failure condition even though past research has constantly shown that Japanese people do not tend to show a self-serving bias.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Kudo|first1=Eriko|last2=Numazaki|first2=Makoto|title=Explicit and Direct Self-Serving Bias in Japan|journal=Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology|volume=34|issue=5|pages=511β521|doi=10.1177/0022022103256475|year=2003|s2cid=146391927}}</ref> Another study conducted by Hugten and Witteloostuijn displayed the results that student participants between the ages of 13 and 15 who mainly process feedback in a non-native English tend to show more self-serving bias than those who process feedback in their native Dutch language.<ref>{{Cite journal|title=The foreign language effect on the selfserving bias: A field experiment in the high school classroom|journal=PLOS ONE|volume=13|issue=2|pages=e0192143|last1=van Hugten|first1=Joeri|last2=van Witteloostuijn|first2=A|date=9 February 2018|doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0192143|pmid=29425224|pmc=5806866|bibcode=2018PLoSO..1392143V|doi-access=free}}</ref> ===Role=== Investigations of self-serving bias distinguish between the role of participants as the actor of a task or as the observer of someone else performing a task, relating closely to [[actorβobserver asymmetry]]. Actors of a task exhibit the self-serving bias in their attributions to their own success or failure feedback, whereas observers do not make the same attributions about another person's task outcome.<ref name="Campbell & Sedikides" /> Observers tend to be more objective in their tendency to ascribe internal or external attributions to other people's outcomes. This may be due to the fact that the self-image of actors is challenged directly and therefore actors feel the need to protect their own self-image, but do not feel the same inclination to do so when the self-image of others is threatened.<ref name="BaumeisterHeatherton1993">{{cite journal|last1=Baumeister|first1=Roy F.|last2=Heatherton|first2=Todd F.|last3=Tice|first3=Dianne M.|title=When ego threats lead to self-regulation failure: Negative consequences of high self-esteem|journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology|volume=64|issue=1|year=1993|pages=141β156|doi=10.1037/0022-3514.64.1.141|pmid=8421250|citeseerx=10.1.1.404.6640|s2cid=770533 }}</ref> ===Self-esteem and emotion=== Emotions can influence feelings of [[self-esteem]], which in turn alters the need to protect one's self-identity. Individuals with higher self-esteem are thought to have more to protect in their self-image, and therefore exhibit the self-serving bias more often than those individuals with lower self-esteem.<ref name="Campbell & Sedikides" /> In a study, participants who were induced to feel the [[emotion]]s of guilt or revulsion were less likely to make self-serving attributions for success and less likely to make self-protecting attributions for failure.<ref name=Coleman>{{cite journal|last=Coleman|first=Martin D.|title=Emotion and the Self-Serving Bias|journal=Current Psychology|year=2011|volume=30|issue=4|pages=345β354|doi=10.1007/s12144-011-9121-2|s2cid=143545174}}</ref> Coleman concluded that the two emotions of guilt and revulsion lead to a drop in self-esteem, and thus a reduction in the use of the self-serving bias. ===Self-awareness and probability of improvement=== The relationship between individuals' awareness levels and perceived probability of improvement also influences the activation of the self-serving bias.<ref name= Duval>{{cite journal|last=Duval|first=Thomas|author2=Paul Silvia |title=Self-Awareness, Probability of Improvement, and the Self-Serving Bias|journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology|date=2002|volume=82|issue=1|pages=49β61|doi=10.1037/0022-3514.82.1.49|pmid=11811633|url=http://libres.uncg.edu/ir/uncg/f/P_Silvia_Self_2005.pdf}}</ref> Individuals with high self-awareness attribute failure internally when they perceive a high probability of improvement. However, they will engage in self-serving bias, attributing failure externally when they perceive a low probability of improvement. Individuals low in self-awareness will attribute failure externally regardless of their perceived probability of improvement. ==Real-world implications== ===Interpersonal relations=== Whether the self-serving bias is exhibited may depend on interpersonal closeness, relationships in a social context. When working in pairs to complete interdependent outcome tasks, relationally close pairs did not show a self-serving bias while relationally distant pairs did.<ref name="Campbell et al." /> A study on self-serving bias in relational context suggests this is due to the idea that close relationships place limits on an individual's self enhancement tendencies.<ref>{{cite journal|title=The Self-Serving Bias in Relational Context|journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology|date=1998|volume=74|issue=2|pages=378β386|doi=10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.378|last1=Sedikides|first1=Constantine|last2=Campbell|first2=W. Keith|last3=Reeder|first3=Glenn D.|last4=Elliot|first4=Andrew J.|s2cid=67763602}}</ref> The individual becomes more modest, when in a close relationship, and is less likely to use that relationship for his or her own benefit. Understanding why partners refrain from the self-serving bias is still in question but can partially be explained by favorable impression those in close relationships have for one another. A similar result was shown when looking at pairs of friends and strangers. Pairs performed an interdependent outcomes creativity test and were then given a bogus pipeline for a success or failure outcome. Strangers exhibited the self-serving bias in responsibility attributions, but friends tended to make joint attributions for both success and failure. Researchers have taken this as evidence for "boundaries on self-enhancement".<ref name="Campbell et al." /> In another study conducted in 2016, the implicit and explicit evaluation of 108 partners and exes as parents who were either married, separated or divorced was researched to investigate if the self-serving bias influenced them.<ref name=":0">{{Cite journal|title=Self-Serving Bias in the Implicit and Explicit Evaluation of Partners and Exes as Parents: A Pilot Study|journal=Psychological Reports|volume=118|issue=1|last=Verrastro|first=Valeria|date=February 1, 2016|pages=251β265|doi=10.1177/0033294115626819|pmid=29693532|s2cid=23484844}}</ref> Using two Implicit Association tests, one measuring Self vs Partner and the other measuring Self vs Ex, results showed that most of the time, men and women consider their exes or partners as less suitable parents which demonstrates the self-serving bias as they "ascribe more easily successes to internal factors" and failures towards their partners.<ref name=":0" /> Also, another result demonstrated that "women revealed a higher self-serving bias than men on both implicit and explicit measures, but only toward exes and not toward current partners" because they consider their exes as an out-group and their partners as an in-group.<ref name=":0" /> ===Workplace=== The self-serving bias can be found in several aspects of the workplace. Research shows that the self-serving bias is used to explain employment: being hired for a job is attributed to personal factors, whereas failure to obtain a job is attributed to external factors.<ref name=Furnham>{{cite journal|last=Furnham, A.|title=Explanations for Unemployment in Britain|journal=European Journal of Social Psychology|year=1982|volume=12|issue=4|pages=335β352|doi=10.1002/ejsp.2420120402}}</ref> Experimental investigation of the explanations for unemployment through asking participants to imagine particular job opportunities and likelihood of getting those jobs, however, did not show such a self-serving bias.<ref name=Pal /> Researchers claim that this may be due to the actor-observer role differences in the self-serving bias. Within the workplace, victims of serious occupational accidents tend to attribute their accidents to external factors, whereas their coworkers and management tend to attribute the accidents to the victims' own actions.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Gyekye|first=Seth Ayim|author2=Salminen, Simo |title=The self-defensive attribution hypothesis in the work environment: Co-workers' perspectives|journal=Safety Science|year=2006|volume=44|issue=2|pages=157β168|doi=10.1016/j.ssci.2005.06.006}}</ref> Interpersonal dynamics of the self-serving bias in the previous section have implications for attributions for outcomes in the workplace. In an investigation of [[group dynamics]], virtual group members had to complete a decision-making task via computer-mediated communication. Results showed that the self-serving bias was present in negative outcomes, and that greater interpersonal distance from group members increased blame for negative outcomes.<ref name=Walther>{{cite journal|last=Walther|first=J. B.|author2=Bazarova, N. N. |title=Misattribution in virtual groups: The effects of member distribution on self-serving bias and partner blame|journal=Human Communication Research|year=2007|volume=33|issue=1|pages=1β26|doi=10.1111/j.1468-2958.2007.00286.x|s2cid=17477072}}</ref> ====Narcissism==== Studies revealed that [[narcissism]] was related to enhanced self-ratings of leadership, even when controlling for the [[Big Five personality traits|Big Five traits]]. Another study showed that narcissism was related to enhanced leadership self-perceptions; indeed, whereas narcissism was significantly positively correlated with self-ratings of leadership, it was significantly negatively related to other ratings of leadership. This study also revealed that narcissism was related to more favorable self-ratings of [[workplace deviance]] and contextual performance compared to other (supervisor) ratings.<ref name=JAP>{{cite journal|title=Loving Yourself Abundantly: Relationship of the Narcissistic Personality to Self- and Other Perceptions of Workplace Deviance, Leadership, and Task and Contextual Performance|url=http://www.timothy-judge.com/Judge,%20LePine,%20and%20Rich%20JAP%202006.pdf|journal=Journal of Applied Psychology|date=2006|volume= 91|issue=4|pages= 762β776|doi=10.1037/0021-9010.91.4.762 |last1=Judge|first1=Timothy A.|last2=Lepine|first2=Jeffery A.|last3=Rich|first3=Bruce L.|pmid=16834504}}</ref> Because narcissism broadly reflects strong self-admiration and behavioral tendencies which may not be viewed positively by others it is possible that narcissism influences self- and other perceptions differently, and insight into this possibility may be important given that differences in perceptions are the foundation for certain types of performance management and development practices.<ref name=JAP/> ===Classroom=== Studies in both lab and field settings have shown both teachers and students hold self-serving biases in regard to outcomes in the classroom.<ref>{{cite journal|last=McAllister|first=Hunter A.|title=Self-serving bias in the classroom: Who shows it? Who knows it?|journal=Journal of Educational Psychology|year=1996|volume=88|issue=1|pages=123β131|doi=10.1037/0022-0663.88.1.123}}</ref> These attributions hold the potential for conflict between teacher and student since neither will take personal responsibility, as the student may blame the teacher while the teacher holds the student accountable. However, both teachers and students also reported being aware of the others' bias, which indicated there may be a feasible conflict resolution mechanism. ===Computer technology=== Computers have become an integral part of everyday life, and research has shown that individuals may subconsciously treat interactions with computers as they would treat a social situation.<ref name="Moon 2000">{{cite journal|last=Moon|first=Youngme|title=Intimate exchanges: Using computers to elicit self-disclosure from consumers|journal=Journal of Consumer Research|year=2000|volume=26|issue=4|pages=324β340|doi=10.1086/209566}}</ref> This finding combined with what is known about the self-serving bias in interpersonal relations indicates that consumers that use a computer to buy products will take personal credit for successful purchases but blame the computer for negative purchase experiences. It was also found, however, that consumers are more willing to attribute successful purchases to the computer and not ascribe blame to the computer for failed purchases if they have "intimate [[self-disclosure]]" with the computer, which Moon describes as revelation of personal information that makes the discloser feel vulnerable.<ref name=Moon /> Another reason is that people are so used to bad functionality, counterintuitive features, bugs, and sudden crashes of most contemporary software applications that they tend not to complain about computer problems. Instead, they believe it is their personal responsibility to predict possible issues and to find solutions to computer problems. This unique phenomenon has been recently observed in several human-computer interaction investigations.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Serenko|first= A.|year=2007|title= Are interface agents scapegoats? Attributions of responsibility in human-agent interaction|journal= Interacting with Computers |volume= 19|issue= 2|pages=293β303|url=http://www.aserenko.com/papers/IwC_Published_Scapegoats_Serenko.pdf|doi=10.1016/j.intcom.2006.07.005}}</ref> ===Sports=== Individuals have been shown to exhibit the self-serving bias in regard to sports outcomes. In one study, collegiate wrestlers at the Division I level made self-reported attributions of the outcomes in their preseason matches. Winners were more likely than losers to attribute the match outcome to internal causes.<ref name=DeMichele /> The researchers note that wrestling is a one-on-one sport and has clearly defined winners. Therefore, other sports of this nature may show similar results, but other team sports or sports with more ambiguous outcomes may not show the same pattern for the self-serving bias.<ref name=DeMichele /> In another study conducted in 1987, the research focused on comparing the self-serving attributions made by individuals who played single sports and those who played in teams. The study gathered 549 statements that were coded for attributional content from lone performers such as tennis and golf players and team performers such as baseball, football or basketball players.<ref name=":1">{{Cite journal|last1=Zaccaro|first1=Stephen J.|last2=Peterson|first2=Christopher|last3=Walker|first3=Steven|date=1987|title=Self-Serving Attributions for Individual and Group Performance|jstor=2786826|journal=Social Psychology Quarterly|volume=50|issue=3|pages=257β263|doi=10.2307/2786826}}</ref> The results showed that "lone performers made more self-serving attributions than team performers" because their performance outcomes have a greater effect on their individual esteem unlike for group outcomes where it "must often be distributed among all participants."<ref name=":1" /> To expand upon self-serving attributions made by team sports, a study conducted in 1980 coded "newspaper accounts of baseball and football for attributional content."<ref name=":2">{{Cite journal|last1=Lau|first1=Richard|last2=Russel|first2=Dan|date=1980|title=Attributions in the Sports Pages|journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology|volume=39|pages=29β38|doi=10.1037/0022-3514.39.1.29|s2cid=145305506}}</ref> The coding of the newspaper accounts showed that there was a "tendency to make internal attributions for success and external attributions for failure" which supports the self-serving bias as about 75% of the attributions from winning teams were internal while about 55% of attributions from losing teams were internal.<ref name=":2" /> ===Depression=== {{See also|Depressive realism}} Clinically depressed patients tend to show less of a self-serving bias than individuals in the general population.<ref name="Greenberg et al" /> In a study exploring the effects of mood on the self-serving bias, the moods of participants were manipulated to be either positive or negative. Negative mood participants were less likely to attribute successful outcomes to the self than positive mood participants, attributing success to external factors.<ref name=Gordon>{{cite journal|last=Gordon|first=R.|author2=Holley, P. |author3=Shaffer, C. |journal=The Journal of Social Psychology|year=2001|volume=130|issue=4|pages=565β567|pmid=2232740|title=Effect of transient mood state on the self-serving bias|doi=10.1080/00224545.1990.9924621}}</ref> It has been suggested that the negative mood in depressed individuals as well as their self-focused attention explains why clinically depressed populations are less likely to exhibit the self-serving bias than normal populations.<ref name="Greenberg et al" /> ===Impacts in negotiation=== {{See also|Negotiation}} Self-Serving bias is defined also by Kaplan et al.<ref name=Kaplan>{{cite journal|last=Kaplan|first=T.|author2=Ruffle, B. |journal=Economic Inquiry|year=2004|pages=237β246|title=The self-serving bias and beliefs about rationality|volume=42|issue=2|doi=10.1093/ei/cbh057|hdl=10036/24400|hdl-access=free}}</ref> to individual's preferences, which effects to his/her beliefs in an optimistic way. Kaplan et al. also defines, that SSB should be termed as βself-defeatingβ phenomenon as it appears for persons, who does not give up. Furthermore, Kaplan et al. state, that specific type of self-serving bias is wishful thinking. This is typically present, when unpleasant surprise pops up in the negotiation, particularly when the opponent has made preparations carefully. Another example is well-known phenomenon from law-court and is commonly used also in law-based TV-series. We can say, that according to Kaplan et al.'s finding, that self-serving bias is playing a major role in negotiation context. There is an emotional effect to the negotiations and there seems to be a reflection between self-serving bias and emotions. ==Neural research outcomes== ===Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)=== The self-serving bias has been investigated by the [[Functional magnetic resonance imaging|fMRI method]] in normal populations. Attributions using the bias show activation in the [[dorsal striatum]], which plays a role in motivated behavior, as well as in the dorsal anterior cingulate.<ref name="Blackwood-2003">{{cite journal|last=Blackwood|first=NJ|author2=Bentall RP |author3=Fytche DH |author4=Simmons A |author5=Murray RM |author6=Howard RJ. |title=Self-responsibility and the self-serving bias: an fMRI investigation of causal attributions|journal=NeuroImage|year=2003|volume=20|issue=2|pages=1076β85|doi=10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00331-8|pmid=14568477|s2cid=37340596}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last=Seidel|first=Eva-Maria|author2=Eickhoff, Simon B. |author3=Kellermann, Thilo |author4=Schneider, Frank |author5=Gur, Ruben C. |author6=Habel, Ute |author7=Derntl, Birgit |title=Who is to blame? Neural correlates of causal attribution in social situations|journal=Social Neuroscience|year=2010|volume=5|issue=4|pages=335β350|doi=10.1080/17470911003615997|pmid=20162490|s2cid=36860601|pmc=8019091}}</ref> In [[major depressive disorder|clinically depressed]] patients, there appear to be weaker connections between the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex and limbic areas of the brain, so this connection may play a role in self-serving attributions.<ref name="Seidel-2011" /> ===Electroencephalography (EEG)=== In a study employing the [[Electroencephalography|EEG]] method of examining brain activation, participants were given bogus outcome feedback that indicated either success or failure and told to make attributions. Different from non-self-serving responses, self-serving responses did not show increased dorsomedial frontal cortex activity preceding attribution decisions. Such lack of brain activity implies that self-control, which is controlled by the dorsomedial frontal cortex, is not as prominent in self-serving attributions as non-self-serving ones.<ref name="Krusemark et al" /> ==See also== {{Portal|Psychology}} <!-- New links innalphabetical order please --> {{columns-list|colwidth=20em| * [[Attribution (psychology)]] * [[Chauvinism]] * [[Choice-supportive bias]] * [[Cognitive bias]] * [[Confidence]] * [[DunningβKruger effect]] * [[Fundamental attribution error]] * [[Grandiose delusions]] * [[Haughtiness]] * [[Hubris]] * [[Icarus complex]] * [[Illusory superiority]] * [[List of cognitive biases]] * [[Marking your own homework]] * [[Moral hazard]] * [[Narcissism]] * [[Omission bias]] * [[Optimism bias]] * [[Overconfidence effect]] * [[Positive illusions]] * [[Self-licensing]] * [[Social projection]] * [[Vanity]]}} ==References== {{Reflist|30em}} {{Biases}} {{Narcissism}} {{DEFAULTSORT:Self-Serving Bias}} [[Category:Cognitive biases]] [[Category:Self]] [[Category:Social psychology]] [[Category:Narcissism]]
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Pages transcluded onto the current version of this page
(
help
)
:
Template:Biases
(
edit
)
Template:Cite encyclopedia
(
edit
)
Template:Cite journal
(
edit
)
Template:Columns-list
(
edit
)
Template:Narcissism
(
edit
)
Template:Portal
(
edit
)
Template:Reflist
(
edit
)
Template:See also
(
edit
)
Template:Short description
(
edit
)