Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Think aloud protocol
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
{{Short description|Method to gather data in usability testing}} A '''think-aloud''' (or '''thinking aloud''') '''protocol''' is a method used to gather data in [[usability testing]] in product design and development, in [[psychology]] and a range of social sciences (e.g., reading, writing, translation research, decision making, and [[process tracing]]). == Description == Think-aloud protocols involve participants thinking aloud as they are performing a set of specified tasks. Participants are asked to say whatever comes into their mind as they complete the task. This might include what they are looking at, thinking, doing, and feeling. This gives observers insight into the participant's cognitive processes (rather than only their final product), to make thought processes as explicit as possible during task performance. In a formal research protocol, all verbalizations are transcribed and then analyzed. In a [[usability testing]] context, observers are asked to take notes of what participants say and do, without attempting to interpret their actions and words, and especially noting places where they encounter difficulty. Test sessions may be completed on participants own devices or in a more controlled setting.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Mclaughlin|first1=Matthew|last2=Duff|first2=Jed|last3=McKenzie|first3=Tom|last4=Campbell|first4=Elizabeth|last5=Sutherland|first5=Rachel|last6=Wiggers|first6=John|last7=Wolfenden|first7=Luke|date=2021-07-26|title=Evaluating Digital Program Support for the Physical Activity 4 Everyone (PA4E1) School Program: Mixed Methods Study|journal=JMIR Pediatrics and Parenting|language=en|volume=4|issue=3|pages=e26690|pmid=34309565| doi=10.2196/26690|issn=2561-6722|pmc=8367175 |doi-access=free }}</ref> Sessions are often audio- and video-recorded so that developers can go back and refer to what participants did and how they reacted.<ref>{{Cite journal|last1=Mclaughlin|first1=Matthew|last2=Duff|first2=Jed|last3=McKenzie|first3=Tom|last4=Campbell|first4=Elizabeth|last5=Sutherland|first5=Rachel|last6=Wiggers|first6=John|last7=Wolfenden|first7=Luke|date=2021-07-26|title=Evaluating Digital Program Support for the Physical Activity 4 Everyone (PA4E1) School Program: Mixed Methods Study|journal=JMIR Pediatrics and Parenting|language=en|volume=4|issue=3|pages=e26690|doi=10.2196/26690|pmid=34309565 |issn=2561-6722|pmc=8367175 |doi-access=free }}</ref> == History == The think-aloud method was introduced in the usability field by Clayton Lewis<ref>{{cite tech report|author=Lewis, C. H.|year=1982|title=Using the "Thinking Aloud" Method In Cognitive Interface Design|institution=IBM|number=RC-9265}}</ref> while he was at [[IBM]], and is explained in ''Task-Centered User Interface Design: A Practical Introduction'' by Lewis and John Rieman.<ref>http://grouplab.cpsc.ucalgary.ca/saul/hci_topics/tcsd-book/chap-1_v-1.html ''Task-Centered User Interface Design: A Practical Introduction'', by Clayton Lewis and John Rieman.</ref> The method was developed based on the techniques of [[protocol analysis]] by K. Ericsson and H. Simon.<ref>{{cite journal|author=Ericsson, K., & Simon, H.|date=May 1980|title=Verbal reports as data|journal=Psychological Review|volume=87|issue=3|pages=215–251|doi=10.1037/0033-295X.87.3.215}}</ref><ref>{{cite book|author=Ericsson, K., & Simon, H.|title=Introspection in Second Language Research|publisher=Multilingual Matters|year=1987|editor1=C. Faerch|location=Clevedon, Avon|pages=24–54|chapter=Verbal reports on thinking|editor2=G. Kasper}}</ref><ref>{{cite book|author=Ericsson, K., & Simon, H.|title=Protocol Analysis: Verbal Reports as Data|publisher=MIT Press|year=1993|isbn=0-262-05029-3|edition=2nd|location=Boston}}</ref> However, there are some significant differences between the way Ericsson and Simon propose that protocols be conducted and how they are actually conducted by usability practitioners, as noted by Ted Boren and Judith Ramey.<ref>https://www.researchgate.net/publication/3230127_Thinking_aloud_Reconciling_theory_and_practice ''Thinking Aloud: Reconciling Theory and Practice'', by Ted Boren and Judith Ramey.</ref> These differences arise from the specific needs and context of usability testing; practitioners should be aware of these differences and adjust their method to meet their needs while still collecting valid data. For example, they may need to prompt for additional information more often than Ericsson and Simon would allow, but should take care not to influence what participants say and do. == Process == A typical procedure of think-aloud protocols would include: * '''Design the study and write the guide:''' Determine the number and type of participant for the study. Generally 5 participants would be sufficient.<ref>{{Cite web|last=Nielson|first=Jakob|title=Why You Only Need to Test with 5 Users|url=https://www.nngroup.com/articles/why-you-only-need-to-test-with-5-users/|website=Nielsen Norman Group}}</ref> The next step is to write a guide that ask the participants to complete the tasks intended with clear step-by-step instructions. In the script, there should be reminders to participants to say their thoughts out when performing tasks. * '''Recruit participants:''' The team should set up a screener for eligibility of participants. After contacting the person of interest and setting up meeting details such as time and location, the team could also provide additional information to help participant better prepare for the activity. * '''Conduct think-aloud protocol:''' After stating the purpose and asking for consent, the team should proceed by giving instructions to the participant. Ask open-ended questions and follow-up questions. The team should avoid asking leading questions or giving clues. * '''Analyze the findings and summarize insights:''' The team should use notes taken during the sessions to generate insights and to find common patterns. Based on the findings, the design team could then decide directions to take action on. As Kuusela and Paul<ref>{{cite journal|author=Kuusela, H., & Paul, P.|year=2000|title=A comparison of concurrent and retrospective verbal protocol analysis|journal=American Journal of Psychology|publisher=University of Illinois Press|volume=113|issue=3|pages=387–404|doi=10.2307/1423365|jstor=1423365|pmid=10997234}}</ref> state, the think-aloud protocol can be distinguished into two different types of experimental procedures. The first is the concurrent think-aloud protocol, collected during the task. The second is the retrospective think-aloud protocol, gathered after the task as the participant walks back through the steps they took previously, often prompted by a video recording of themselves. There are benefits and drawbacks to each approach, but in general a concurrent protocol may be more complete, while a retrospective protocol has less chance to interfere with task performance. Nonetheless, some concurrent protocols have not produced such interference effects,<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Byrd |first1=Nick |last2=Joseph |first2=Brianna |last3=Gongora |first3=Gabriela |last4=Sirota |first4=Miroslav |title=Tell Us What You Really Think: A Think Aloud Protocol Analysis of the Verbal Cognitive Reflection Test |journal=Journal of Intelligence |date=2023 |volume=11 |issue=4 |page=76 |doi=10.3390/jintelligence11040076 |pmid=37103261 |pmc=10146599 |doi-access=free }}</ref> suggesting that it may be possible to optimize both completeness and authenticity of verbal reports. == Benefits == The think-aloud method allows researchers to discover what users genuinely think of your design.<ref name=":0">{{Cite web|last=Experience|first=World Leaders in Research-Based User|title=Thinking Aloud: The #1 Usability Tool|url=https://www.nngroup.com/articles/thinking-aloud-the-1-usability-tool/|access-date=2021-12-13|website=Nielsen Norman Group|language=en}}</ref> == Related method == A related but slightly different data-gathering method is the '''talk-aloud protocol'''. This involves participants only describing their actions but not other thoughts. This method is thought to be more objective in that participants merely report how they go about completing a task rather than interpreting or justifying their actions (see the standard works{{Which|date=August 2019}} by Ericsson & Simon).{{Cn|date=August 2019}} == See also == {{Portal|Psychology}} * [[Comparison of usability evaluation methods]] * [[Partial concurrent thinking aloud]] * [[Protocol analysis]] * [[Retrospective think aloud]] * [[Rubber duck debugging]] ==References== {{reflist}} ==External links== * [https://www.nngroup.com/articles/thinking-aloud-the-1-usability-tool/ Thinking Aloud: The #1 Usability Tool] - an introduction to the think aloud method * [https://www.gov.uk/service-manual/user-research/using-moderated-usability-testing Using moderated usability testing] - UK government resource on using think aloud {{DEFAULTSORT:Think Aloud Protocol}} <!--Categories--> [[Category:Human–computer interaction]] [[Category:Software testing]] [[Category:Usability]]
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Pages transcluded onto the current version of this page
(
help
)
:
Template:Cite book
(
edit
)
Template:Cite journal
(
edit
)
Template:Cite tech report
(
edit
)
Template:Cite web
(
edit
)
Template:Cn
(
edit
)
Template:Portal
(
edit
)
Template:Reflist
(
edit
)
Template:Short description
(
edit
)
Template:Which
(
edit
)