Open main menu
Home
Random
Recent changes
Special pages
Community portal
Preferences
About Wikipedia
Disclaimers
Incubator escapee wiki
Search
User menu
Talk
Dark mode
Contributions
Create account
Log in
Editing
Transformational grammar
Warning:
You are not logged in. Your IP address will be publicly visible if you make any edits. If you
log in
or
create an account
, your edits will be attributed to your username, along with other benefits.
Anti-spam check. Do
not
fill this in!
{{Short description|Earliest model of generative grammar}} In [[linguistics]], '''transformational grammar''' ('''TG''') or '''transformational-generative grammar''' ('''TGG''') was the earliest [[scientific modelling|model]] of [[grammar]] proposed within the research tradition of [[generative grammar]].<ref name ="WasowHandbooklede">{{cite encyclopedia |title=Generative Grammar |encyclopedia=The Handbook of Linguistics|year=2003|last=Wasow|first=Thomas|author-link=Tom Wasow|editor-last1=Aronoff|editor-first1=Mark|editor-last2=Ress-Miller|editor-first2=Janie|publisher= Blackwell|url=https://www.blackwellpublishing.com/content/BPL_Images/Content_store/WWW_Content/9780631204978/12.pdf|doi=10.1002/9780470756409.ch12|quote=Early generative work was known as "transformational grammar"}}</ref> Like current generative theories, it treated grammar as a system of [[formation rule|formal rules]] that generate all and only [[grammaticality|grammatical]] sentences of a given language. What was distinctive about transformational grammar was that it posited '''transformation rules''' that mapped a sentence's [[deep structure]] to its pronounced form. For example, in many variants of transformational grammar, the [[English language|English]] [[active voice|active]] voice sentence "Emma saw Daisy" and its [[passive voice|passive]] counterpart "Daisy was seen by Emma" share a common deep structure generated by [[phrase structure rules]], differing only in that the latter's structure is modified by a passivization transformation rule. == Basic mechanisms == Transformational grammar was a species of [[generative grammar]] and shared many of its goals and postulations, including the notion of linguistics as a [[cognitive science]], the need for [[formal language|formal explicitness]], and the [[linguistic competence|competence]]-[[linguistic performance|performance]] distinction.<ref name ="WasowHandbook"/> Transformational grammar included two kinds of rules: phrase-structure rules and transformational rules. ===Deep structure and surface structure=== {{linguistics}} {{main article|Deep structure and surface structure}} In transformational grammar, each sentence in a language has two levels of representation: a deep structure and a surface structure.<ref name="aspects">{{cite book|title=Aspects of the Theory of Syntax|publisher=MIT Press|author=Chomsky, Noam|year=1965|isbn=0-262-53007-4|url=https://archive.org/details/aspectsoftheoryo00chom}}</ref> The deep structure represents a sentence's core [[semantics|semantic relations]] and is mapped onto the surface structure, which follows the sentence's [[phonology|phonological system]] very closely, via ''transformations''. Deep structures are generated by [[phrase structure grammar]]s using [[rewriting|rewrite rules]]. ===Transformations=== Transformations are rules that map a deep structure to a surface structure. For example, a typical transformation in TG is [[subject-auxiliary inversion]] (SAI). That rule takes as its input a declarative sentence with an auxiliary, such as "John has eaten all the heirloom tomatoes", and transforms it into "Has John eaten all the heirloom tomatoes?" In the original formulation (Chomsky 1957), those rules held over strings of terminals, constituent symbols or both. :X NP AUX Y <math>\Rightarrow</math> X AUX NP Y (NP = Noun Phrase and AUX = Auxiliary) In the 1970s, by the time of the Extended Standard Theory, following Joseph Emonds's work on structure preservation, transformations came to be viewed as holding over trees. By the end of government and binding theory, in the late 1980s, transformations were no longer structure-changing operations at all; instead, they added information to already existing trees by copying constituents. The earliest conceptions of transformations were that they were construction-specific devices. For example, there was a transformation that turned active sentences into passive ones. A different transformation raised embedded subjects into main clause subject position in sentences such as "John seems to have gone", and a third reordered arguments in the dative alternation. With the shift from rules to principles and constraints in the 1970s, those construction-specific transformations morphed into general rules (all the examples just mentioned are instances of NP movement), which eventually changed into the single general rule [[Move α|move alpha]] or Move. Transformations actually come in two types: the post-deep structure kind mentioned above, which are string- or structure-changing, and generalized transformations (GTs). GTs were originally proposed in the earliest forms of generative grammar (such as in Chomsky 1957). They take small structures, either atomic or generated by other rules, and combine them. For example, the generalized transformation of embedding would take the kernel "Dave said X" and the kernel "Dan likes smoking" and combine them into "Dave said Dan likes smoking." GTs are thus structure-building rather than structure-changing. In the Extended Standard Theory and [[government and binding theory]], GTs were abandoned in favor of recursive phrase structure rules, but they are still present in [[tree-adjoining grammar]] as the Substitution and Adjunction operations, and have recently reemerged in mainstream generative grammar in Minimalism, as the operations Merge and Move. In generative [[phonology]], another form of transformation is the [[phonological rule]], which describes a mapping between an [[underlying representation]] (the [[phoneme]]) and the surface form that is articulated during [[Spoken language|natural speech]].<ref>{{cite book|last=Goldsmith|first=John A|title=The Handbook of Phonological Theory|publisher=Blackwell Publishers|year=1995|isbn=1-4051-5768-2|editor=John A. Goldsmith|series=Blackwell Handbooks in Linguistics|page=2|chapter=Phonological Theory|author-link=John Goldsmith (linguist)}}</ref> ====Mathematical representation==== An important feature of all transformational grammars is that they are more powerful than [[context-free grammar]]s.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Peters|first=Stanley|author2=R. Ritchie|year=1973|title=On the generative power of transformational grammars|url=http://www.cs.utoronto.ca/~gpenn/csc2517/peters-ritchie73.pdf|journal=Information Sciences|volume=6|pages=49–83|doi=10.1016/0020-0255(73)90027-3}}</ref> Chomsky formalized this idea in the [[Chomsky hierarchy]]. He argued that it is impossible to describe the structure of natural languages with context-free grammars.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Chomsky|first=Noam|year=1956|title=Three models for the description of language|url=http://www.chomsky.info/articles/195609--.pdf|url-status=dead|journal=IRE Transactions on Information Theory|volume=2|issue=3|pages=113–124|doi=10.1109/TIT.1956.1056813|s2cid=19519474 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20100919021754/http://chomsky.info/articles/195609--.pdf|archive-date=2010-09-19}}</ref> His general position on the context-dependency of natural language has held up, though his specific examples of the inadequacy of CFGs in terms of their weak generative capacity were disproved.<ref>{{cite journal|last=Shieber|first=Stuart|year=1985|title=Evidence against the context-freeness of natural language|url=http://www.eecs.harvard.edu/~shieber/Biblio/Papers/shieber85.pdf|journal=Linguistics and Philosophy|volume=8|issue=3|pages=333–343|doi=10.1007/BF00630917|s2cid=222277837}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last=Pullum|first=Geoffrey K.|author2=Gerald Gazdar|year=1982|title=Natural languages and context-free languages|journal=Linguistics and Philosophy|volume=4|issue=4|pages=471–504|doi=10.1007/BF00360802|s2cid=189881482}}</ref> ==Core concepts== ===Innate linguistic knowledge=== Using a term such as "transformation" may give the impression that theories of transformational generative grammar are intended as a model of the processes by which the human mind constructs and understands sentences, but Chomsky clearly stated that a generative grammar models only the knowledge that underlies the human ability to speak and understand, arguing that because most of that knowledge is innate, a baby can have a large body of knowledge about the structure of language in general and so need to ''learn'' only the idiosyncratic features of the language(s) to which it is exposed.{{Citation needed|date=October 2021}} Chomsky is not the first person to suggest that all languages have certain fundamental things in common. He quoted philosophers who posited the same basic idea several centuries ago. But Chomsky helped make the innateness theory respectable after a period dominated by more behaviorist attitudes towards language. He made concrete and technically sophisticated proposals about the structure of language as well as important proposals about how grammatical theories' success should be evaluated.<ref>{{cite web |last1=McLeod |first1=S |title=Language Acquisition |url=https://www.simplypsychology.org/language.html |website=Simply Psychology |access-date=21 February 2019}}</ref> ===Grammaticality=== {{Further|Grammaticality}} Chomsky argued that "grammatical" and "ungrammatical" can be meaningfully and usefully defined. In contrast, an extreme behaviorist linguist would argue that language can be studied only through recordings or transcriptions of actual speech and that the role of the linguist is to look for patterns in such observed speech, not to hypothesize about why such patterns might occur or to label particular utterances grammatical or ungrammatical. Few linguists in the 1950s actually took such an extreme position, but Chomsky was on the opposite extreme, defining grammaticality in an unusually [[mentalism (psychology)|mentalistic]] way for the time.<ref>{{cite book|author=Newmeyer, Frederick J.|title=Linguistic Theory in America|publisher=Academic Press|year=1986|edition=Second}}{{page needed|date=November 2013}}</ref> He argued that the intuition of a [[native speaker]] is enough to define the grammaticality of a sentence; that is, if a particular string of English words elicits a double-take or a feeling of wrongness in a native English speaker, with various extraneous factors affecting intuitions controlled for, it can be said that the string of words is ungrammatical. That, according to Chomsky, is entirely distinct from the question of whether a sentence is meaningful or can be understood. It is possible for a sentence to be both grammatical and meaningless, as in Chomsky's famous example, "[[colorless green ideas sleep furiously]]".<ref>Chomsky 1957:15</ref> But such sentences manifest a linguistic problem that is distinct from that posed by meaningful but ungrammatical (non)-sentences such as "man the bit sandwich the", the meaning of which is fairly clear, but which no [[native speaker]] would accept as well-formed. ===Theory evaluation=== In the 1960s, Chomsky introduced two central ideas relevant to the construction and evaluation of grammatical theories. ==== Competence versus performance ==== One was the distinction between ''[[Linguistic competence|competence]]'' and ''[[Linguistic performance|performance]]''. Chomsky noted that when people speak in the real world, they often make linguistic errors, such as starting a sentence and then abandoning it midway through. He argued that such errors in linguistic ''performance'' are irrelevant to the study of linguistic ''competence'', the knowledge that allows people to construct and understand grammatical sentences. Consequently, the linguist can study an idealised version of language, which greatly simplifies linguistic analysis. ==== Descriptive versus explanatory adequacy ==== The other idea related directly to evaluation of theories of grammar. Chomsky distinguished between grammars that achieve ''descriptive adequacy'' and those that go further and achieve ''explanatory adequacy''. A descriptively adequate grammar for a particular language defines the (infinite) set of grammatical sentences in that language; that is, it describes the language in its entirety. A grammar that achieves explanatory adequacy has the additional property that it gives insight into the mind's underlying linguistic structures. In other words, it does not merely describe the grammar of a language, but makes predictions about how linguistic knowledge is mentally represented. For Chomsky, such mental representations are largely innate and so if a grammatical theory has explanatory adequacy, it must be able to explain different languages' grammatical nuances as relatively minor variations in the universal pattern of human language. Chomsky argued that even though linguists were still a long way from constructing descriptively adequate grammars, progress in descriptive adequacy would come only if linguists held explanatory adequacy as their goal: real insight into individual languages' structure can be gained only by comparative study of a wide range of languages, on the assumption that they are all cut from the same cloth.{{citation needed|date=November 2013}} ==Historical context== Chomsky developed transformational grammar in the late 1950s, drawing on older work including that of the [[structuralist linguistics|structuralists]].<ref>{{cite book|author=Newmeyer, Frederick J.|author-link=Frederick Newmeyer|title=Linguistic Theory in America|publisher=Academic Press|year=1986|edition=Second|at=Chapter 2,Chapter3}}</ref><ref name ="WasowHandbook">{{cite encyclopedia |title=Generative Grammar|encyclopedia=The Handbook of Linguistics|year=2003|last=Wasow|first=Thomas|author-link=Tom Wasow|editor-last1=Aronoff|editor-first1=Mark|editor-last2=Ress-Miller|editor-first2=Janie|publisher= Blackwell|url=https://www.blackwellpublishing.com/content/BPL_Images/Content_store/WWW_Content/9780631204978/12.pdf|doi=10.1002/9780470756409.ch12}}</ref> Its central ideas are maintained to varying degrees in present-day approaches to syntax such as [[Minimalist program|Minimalism]], while others such as [[Combinatory categorial grammar]] are distinctly non-transformational.<ref name="Partee3">{{cite book |last=Partee |first=Barbara|author-link=Barbara Partee |title=The Baltic International Yearbook of Cognition, Logic and Communication |publisher=BIYCLC |year=2011 |volume=6 |pages=1–52 |chapter=Formal Semantics: Origins, Issues, Early Impact |doi=10.4148/biyclc.v6i0.1580}}</ref> ==See also== * [[Antisymmetry]] * [[Biolinguistics]] * [[Generalised phrase structure grammar]] * [[Generative semantics]] * [[Head-driven phrase structure grammar]] * [[Heavy NP shift]] * [[Jerzy Kuryłowicz]] * [[Lexical functional grammar]] * [[Minimalist program]] * [[Parasitic gap]] * [[Structural linguistics]] * [[Transformational syntax]] ==References== {{Reflist}} ==Bibliography== * {{citation |last=Chomsky |first=Noam |author-link= Noam Chomsky |title=Syntactic Structures |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC |year=1957 |publisher=Mouton |location=The Hague/Paris |isbn=9783110172799 }} * {{cite book|title=The Minimalist Program|author=Chomsky, Noam|publisher=MIT Press|year=1995|isbn=0-262-53128-3}} * {{Cite book|title=The linguistics studentʻs handbook|last=Bauer|first=Laurie|publisher=Edinburgh University Press|year=2007|isbn=978-0-7486-2758-5|pages=47–55}} * {{cite journal | title=Review Article: The Minimalist Program | year=1998 | last=Zwart | first=Jan-Wouter | journal=Journal of Linguistics | volume=34 | pages=213–226 |publisher= Cambridge University Press| doi=10.1017/S0022226797006889 | s2cid=1647815 }} ==External links== * [http://linguistics.concordia.ca/i-language/ What is I-language?] - Chapter 1 of I-language: An Introduction to Linguistics as Cognitive Science. * [http://www.ling.upenn.edu/~beatrice/syntax-textbook/index.html The Syntax of Natural Language] – an online textbook on transformational grammar. * {{cite book | first=Daniela | last=Isac | author2=Charles Reiss | title=I-language: An Introduction to Linguistics as Cognitive Science, 2nd edition | url=https://archive.org/details/ilanguageintrodu00dani | year=2013 | publisher=Oxford University Press | isbn=978-0-19-953420-3 }} {{Authority control}} {{DEFAULTSORT:Transformational Grammar}} [[Category:Generative syntax]] [[Category:Grammar frameworks]] [[Category:Noam Chomsky]] [[Category:Syntactic transformation|*]]
Edit summary
(Briefly describe your changes)
By publishing changes, you agree to the
Terms of Use
, and you irrevocably agree to release your contribution under the
CC BY-SA 4.0 License
and the
GFDL
. You agree that a hyperlink or URL is sufficient attribution under the Creative Commons license.
Cancel
Editing help
(opens in new window)
Pages transcluded onto the current version of this page
(
help
)
:
Template:Authority control
(
edit
)
Template:Citation
(
edit
)
Template:Citation needed
(
edit
)
Template:Cite book
(
edit
)
Template:Cite encyclopedia
(
edit
)
Template:Cite journal
(
edit
)
Template:Cite web
(
edit
)
Template:Further
(
edit
)
Template:Linguistics
(
edit
)
Template:Main article
(
edit
)
Template:Page needed
(
edit
)
Template:Reflist
(
edit
)
Template:Short description
(
edit
)