Magnetoreception
Template:Short description Template:Redirect Template:Good article Template:Use dmy dates
Magnetoreception is a sense which allows an organism to detect the Earth's magnetic field. Animals with this sense include some arthropods, molluscs, and vertebrates (fish, amphibians, reptiles, birds, and mammals). The sense is mainly used for orientation and navigation, but it may help some animals to form regional maps. Experiments on migratory birds provide evidence that they make use of a cryptochrome protein in the eye, relying on the quantum radical pair mechanism to perceive magnetic fields. This effect is extremely sensitive to weak magnetic fields, and readily disturbed by radio-frequency interference, unlike a conventional iron compass.
Birds have iron-containing materials in their upper beaks. There is some evidence that this provides a magnetic sense, mediated by the trigeminal nerve, but the mechanism is unknown.
Cartilaginous fish including sharks and stingrays can detect small variations in electric potential with their electroreceptive organs, the ampullae of Lorenzini. These appear to be able to detect magnetic fields by induction. There is some evidence that these fish use magnetic fields in navigation.
HistoryEdit
Biologists have long wondered whether migrating animals such as birds and sea turtles have an inbuilt magnetic compass, enabling them to navigate using the Earth's magnetic field. Until late in the 20th century, evidence for this was essentially only behavioural: many experiments demonstrated that animals could indeed derive information from the magnetic field around them, but gave no indication of the mechanism. In 1972, Roswitha and Wolfgang Wiltschko showed that migratory birds responded to the direction and inclination (dip) of the magnetic field. In 1977, M. M. Walker and colleagues identified iron-based (magnetite) magnetoreceptors in the snouts of rainbow trout. In 2003, G. Fleissner and colleagues found iron-based receptors in the upper beaks of homing pigeons, both seemingly connected to the animal's trigeminal nerve. Research took a different direction in 2000, however, when Thorsten Ritz and colleagues suggested that a photoreceptor protein in the eye, cryptochrome, was a magnetoreceptor, working at a molecular scale by quantum entanglement.<ref name="Winklhofer 2010">Template:Cite journal</ref>
Proposed mechanismsEdit
In animalsEdit
In animals, the mechanism for magnetoreception is still under investigation. Two main hypotheses are currently being discussed: one proposing a quantum compass based on a radical pair mechanism,<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> the other postulating a more conventional iron-based magnetic compass with magnetite particles.<ref name=Wiltschkojcp>Template:Cite journal</ref>
CryptochromeEdit
According to the first model, magnetoreception is possible via the radical pair mechanism,<ref name="Hore 2016">Template:Cite journal</ref> which is well-established in spin chemistry. The mechanism requires two molecules, each with unpaired electrons, at a suitable distance from each other. When these can exist in states either with their spin axes in the same direction, or in opposite directions, the molecules oscillate rapidly between the two states. That oscillation is extremely sensitive to magnetic fields.<ref name="Rodgers 2009">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="Steiner Ulrich 1989">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="Woodward 2002">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="Rodgers Hore 2009" /> Because the Earth's magnetic field is extremely weak, at 0.5 gauss, the radical pair mechanism is currently the only credible way that the Earth's magnetic field could cause chemical changes (as opposed to the mechanical forces which would be detected via magnetic crystals acting like a compass needle).<ref name="Rodgers Hore 2009">Template:Cite journal</ref>
In 1978, Schulten and colleagues proposed that this was the mechanism of magnetoreception.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> In 2000, scientists proposed that cryptochrome – a flavoprotein in the rod cells in the eyes of birds – was the "magnetic molecule" behind this effect.<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref> It is the only protein known to form photoinduced radical-pairs in animals.<ref name="Hore 2016" /> The function of cryptochrome varies by species, but its mechanism is always the same: exposure to blue light excites an electron in a chromophore, which causes the formation of a radical-pair whose electrons are quantum entangled, enabling the precision needed for magnetoreception.<ref name="Wiltschko Ahmad Nießner Gehring 2016" /><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
Many lines of evidence point to cryptochrome and radical pairs as the mechanism of magnetoreception in birds:<ref name="Hore Mouritsen 2022">Template:Cite journal</ref>
- Despite 20 years of searching, no biomolecule other than cryptochrome has been identified capable of supporting radical pairs.<ref name="Hore Mouritsen 2022" />
- In cryptochrome, a yellow molecule flavin adenine dinucleotide (FAD) can absorb a photon of blue light, putting the cryptochrome into an activated state: an electron is transferred from a tryptophan amino acid to the FAD molecule, forming a radical pair.<ref name="Hore Mouritsen 2022" />
- Of the six types of cryptochrome in birds, cryptochrome-4a (Cry4a) binds FAD much more tightly than the rest.<ref name="Hore Mouritsen 2022" />
- Cry4a levels in migratory birds, which rely on navigation for their survival, are highest during the spring and autumn migration periods, when navigation is most critical.<ref name="Hore Mouritsen 2022" />
- The Cry4a protein from the European robin, a migratory bird, is much more sensitive to magnetic fields than similar but not identical Cry4a from pigeons and chickens, which are non-migratory.<ref name="Hore Mouritsen 2022" />
These findings together suggest that the Cry4a of migratory birds has been selected for its magnetic sensitivity.<ref name="Hore Mouritsen 2022" />
Behavioral experiments on migratory birds support this theory. Caged migratory birds such as robins display migratory restlessness, known by ethologists as Zugunruhe, in spring and autumn: they often orient themselves in the direction in which they would migrate. In 2004, Thorsten Ritz showed that a weak radio-frequency electromagnetic field, chosen to be at the same frequency as the singlet-triplet oscillation of cryptochrome radical pairs, effectively interfered with the birds' orientation. The field would not have interfered with an iron-based compass. Further, birds are unable to detect a 180 degree reversal of the magnetic field, something they would straightforwardly detect with an iron-based compass.<ref name="Hore Mouritsen 2022" />
From 2007 onwards, Henrik Mouritsen attempted to replicate this experiment. Instead, he found that robins were unable to orient themselves in the wooden huts he used. Suspecting extremely weak radio-frequency interference from other electrical equipment on the campus, he tried shielding the huts with aluminium sheeting, which blocks electrical noise but not magnetic fields. When he earthed the sheeting, the robins oriented correctly; when the earthing was removed, the robins oriented at random. Finally, when the robins were tested in a hut far from electrical equipment, the birds oriented correctly. These effects imply a radical-pair compass, not an iron one.<ref name="Hore Mouritsen 2022"/>
In 2016, Wiltschko and colleagues showed that European robins were unaffected by local anaesthesia of the upper beak, showing that in these test conditions orientation was not from iron-based receptors in the beak. In their view, cryptochrome and its radical pairs provide the only model that can explain the avian magnetic compass.<ref name="Wiltschko Ahmad Nießner Gehring 2016">Template:Cite journal A supplement to the paper summarizes alternative hypotheses on avian compass mechanisms.</ref> A scheme with three radicals rather than two has been proposed as more resistant to spin relaxation and explaining the observed behaviour better.<ref name="Kattnig 2017">Template:Cite journal</ref>
Iron-basedEdit
The second proposed model for magnetoreception relies on clusters composed of iron, a natural mineral with strong magnetism, used by magnetotactic bacteria. Iron clusters have been observed in the upper beak of homing pigeons,<ref name="Fleissner2003">Template:Cite journal</ref> and other taxa.<ref name="Falkenberg2010">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="Hore 2016"/><ref name="Solov'yov Greiner 2007">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="Treiber2012">Template:Cite journal</ref> Iron-based systems could form a magnetoreceptive basis for many species including turtles.<ref name="Rodgers Hore 2009" /> Both the exact location and ultrastructure of birds' iron-containing magnetoreceptors remain unknown; they are believed to be in the upper beak, and to be connected to the brain by the trigeminal nerve. This system is in addition to the cryptochrome system in the retina of birds. Iron-based systems of unknown function might also exist in other vertebrates.<ref name="Kishkinev Chernetsov 2015">Template:Cite journal</ref>
Electromagnetic inductionEdit
Another possible mechanism of magnetoreception in animals is electromagnetic induction in cartilaginous fish, namely sharks, stingrays, and chimaeras. These fish have electroreceptive organs, the ampullae of Lorenzini, which can detect small variations in electric potential. The organs are mucus-filled and consist of canals that connect pores in the skin of the mouth and nose to small sacs within the animal's flesh. They are used to sense the weak electric fields of prey and predators. These organs have been predicted to sense magnetic fields, by means of Faraday's law of induction: as a conductor moves through a magnetic field an electric potential is generated. In this case the conductor is the animal moving through a magnetic field, and the potential induced (Vind) depends on the time (t)-varying rate of magnetic flux (Φ) through the conductor according to <math display="block">V_{ind}=-\frac{d\phi}{dt}</math>
The ampullae of Lorenzini detect very small fluctuations in the potential difference between the pore and the base of the electroreceptor sac. An increase in potential results in a decrease in the rate of nerve activity. This is analogous to the behavior of a current-carrying conductor.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="Anderson Clegg Véras Holland 2017" /> Sandbar sharks, Carcharinus plumbeus, have been shown to be able to detect magnetic fields; the experiments provided non-definitive evidence that the animals had a magnetoreceptor, rather than relying on induction and electroreceptors.<ref name="Anderson Clegg Véras Holland 2017 ">Template:Cite journal</ref> Electromagnetic induction has not been studied in non-aquatic animals.<ref name="Rodgers Hore 2009" />
The yellow stingray, Urobatis jamaicensis, is able to distinguish between the intensity and inclination angle of a magnetic field in the laboratory. This suggests that cartilaginous fishes may use the Earth's magnetic field for navigation.<ref name="Newton Gill Kajiura 2020">Template:Cite journal</ref>
Passive alignment in bacteriaEdit
Magnetotactic bacteria of multiple taxa contain sufficient magnetic material in the form of magnetosomes, nanometer-sized particles of magnetite,<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> that the Earth's magnetic field passively aligns them, just as it does with a compass needle. The bacteria are thus not actually sensing the magnetic field.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
A possible but unexplored mechanism of magnetoreception in animals is through endosymbiosis with magnetotactic bacteria, whose DNA is widespread in animals. This would involve having these bacteria living inside an animal, and their magnetic alignment being used as part of a magnetoreceptive system.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
Unanswered questionsEdit
It remains likely that two or more complementary mechanisms play a role in magnetic field detection in animals. Of course, this potential dual mechanism theory raises the questions of to what degree each method is responsible for the stimulus, and how they produce a signal in response to the weak magnetic field of the Earth.<ref name="Rodgers Hore 2009" />
In addition, it is possible that magnetic senses may be different for different species. Some species may only be able to detect north and south, while others may only be able to differentiate between the equator and the poles. Although the ability to sense direction is important in migratory navigation, many animals have the ability to sense small fluctuations in earth's magnetic field to map their position to within a few kilometers.<ref name="Rodgers Hore 2009" /><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
Taxonomic rangeEdit
Magnetoreception is widely distributed taxonomically. It is present in many of the animals so far investigated. These include arthropods, molluscs, and among vertebrates in fish, amphibians, reptiles, birds, and mammals. Its status in other groups remains unknown.<ref name="Wiltschko">Template:Cite book</ref>
The ability to detect and respond to magnetic fields may exist in plants, possibly as in animals mediated by cryptochrome. Experiments by different scientists have identified multiple effects, including changes to growth rate, seed germination, mitochondrial structure, and responses to gravity (geotropism). The results have sometimes been controversial, and no mechanism has been definitely identified. The ability may be widely distributed, but its taxonomic range in plants is unknown.<ref name="Maffei 2014">Template:Cite journal</ref>
In molluscsEdit
The giant sea slug Tochuina gigantea (formerly T. tetraquetra), a mollusc, orients its body between north and east prior to a full moon.<ref name="Lohmann Willows 1987">Template:Cite journal</ref> A 1991 experiment offered a right turn to geomagnetic south and a left turn to geomagnetic east (a Y-shaped maze). 80% of Tochuina made a turn to magnetic east. When the field was reversed, the animals displayed no preference for either turn.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="Wang Cain Lohmann pp. 1043–1049" /> TochuinaTemplate:'s nervous system is composed of individually identifiable neurons, four of which are stimulated by changes in the applied magnetic field, and two which are inhibited by such changes.<ref name="Wang Cain Lohmann pp. 1043–1049">Template:Cite journal</ref> The tracks of the similar species Tritonia exsulans become more variable in direction when close to strong rare-earth magnets placed in their natural habitat, suggesting that the animal uses its magnetic sense continuously to help it travel in a straight line.<ref name="Wyeth Holden Jalala Murray 2021">Template:Cite journal</ref>
In insectsEdit
The fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster may be able to orient to magnetic fields. In one choice test, flies were loaded into an apparatus with two arms that were surrounded by electric coils. Current was run through each of the coils, but only one was configured to produce a 5-Gauss magnetic field (about ten times stronger than the Earth's magnetic field) at a time. The flies were trained to associate the magnetic field with a sucrose reward. Flies with an altered cryptochrome, such as with an antisense mutation, were not sensitive to magnetic fields.<ref name="Gegear2008">Template:Cite journal</ref>
Magnetoreception has been studied in detail in insects including honey bees, ants and termites.<ref name="Pereira-Bomfim 2015">Template:Cite journal</ref> Ants and bees navigate using their magnetic sense both locally (near their nests) and when migrating.<ref name="Wajnberg 2010">Template:Cite journal</ref> In particular, the Brazilian stingless bee Schwarziana quadripunctata is able to detect magnetic fields using the thousands of hair-like sensilla on its antennae.<ref name="Esquivel 2005">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="Lucano 2005">Template:Cite journal</ref>
In vertebratesEdit
In fishEdit
Studies of magnetoreception in bony fish have been conducted mainly with salmon. Both sockeye salmon (Oncorhynchus nerka) and Chinook salmon (Oncorhynchus tschawytscha) have a compass sense. This was demonstrated in experiments in the 1980s by changing the axis of a magnetic field around a circular tank of young fish; they reoriented themselves in line with the field.<ref name="Quinn 1980">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="Taylor 1986">Template:Cite journal</ref>
In amphibiansEdit
Some of the earliest studies of amphibian magnetoreception were conducted with cave salamanders (Eurycea lucifuga). Researchers housed groups of cave salamanders in corridors aligned with either magnetic north–south, or magnetic east–west. In tests, the magnetic field was experimentally rotated by 90°, and salamanders were placed in cross-shaped structures (one corridor along the new north–south axis, one along the new east–west axis). The salamanders responded to the field's rotation.<ref name=":Phillips 1977">Template:Cite journal</ref>
Red-spotted newts (Notophthalmus viridescens) respond to drastic increases in water temperature by heading for land. The behaviour is disrupted if the magnetic field is experimentally altered, showing that the newts use the field for orientation.<ref name="Phillips 1986 newt">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="Phillips 1986 salamander">Template:Cite journal</ref>
Both European toads (Bufo bufo) and natterjack toads (Epidalea calamita) toads rely on vision and olfaction when migrating to breeding sites, but magnetic fields may also play a role. When randomly displaced Template:Convert from their breeding sites, these toads can navigate their way back,<ref name=":12">Template:Cite journal</ref> but this ability can be disrupted by fitting them with small magnets.<ref name=":6">Template:Cite journal</ref>
In reptilesEdit
The majority of study on magnetoreception in reptiles involves turtles. Early support for magnetoreception in turtles was provided in a 1991 study on hatchling loggerhead turtles which demonstrated that loggerheads can use the magnetic field as a compass to determine direction.<ref name="Lohmann 1991">Template:Cite journal</ref> Subsequent studies have demonstrated that loggerhead and green turtles can also use the magnetic field of the earth as a map, because different parameters of the Earth's magnetic field vary with geographic location. The map in sea turtles was the first ever described though similar abilities have now been reported in lobsters, fish, and birds.<ref name="Lohmann et al 2022">Template:Cite journal</ref> Magnetoreception by land turtles was shown in a 2010 experiment on Terrapene carolina, a box turtle. After teaching a group of these box turtles to swim to either the east or west end of an experimental tank, a strong magnet disrupted the learned routes.<ref name="Mathis Moore 2010">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="Stehli 1996">Template:Cite book</ref>
Orientation toward the sea, as seen in turtle hatchlings, may rely partly on magnetoreception. In loggerhead and leatherback turtles, breeding takes place on beaches, and, after hatching, offspring crawl rapidly to the sea. Although differences in light density seem to drive this behaviour, magnetic alignment appears to play a part. For instance, the natural directional preferences held by these hatchlings (which lead them from beaches to the sea) reverse upon experimental inversion of the magnetic poles.<ref name="Merrill Salmon 2010">Template:Cite journal</ref>
In birdsEdit
Homing pigeons use magnetic fields as part of their complex navigation system.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> William Keeton showed that time-shifted homing pigeons (acclimatised in the laboratory to a different time-zone) are unable to orient themselves correctly on a clear, sunny day; this is attributed to time-shifted pigeons being unable to compensate accurately for the movement of the sun during the day. Conversely, time-shifted pigeons released on overcast days navigate correctly, suggesting that pigeons can use magnetic fields to orient themselves; this ability can be disrupted with magnets attached to the birds' backs.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="Gould, J. L. 1984">Template:Cite journal</ref> Pigeons can detect magnetic anomalies as weak as 1.86 gauss.<ref name="ReferenceA">Template:Cite journal</ref>
For a long time the trigeminal system was the suggested location for a magnetite-based magnetoreceptor in the pigeon. This was based on two findings: First, magnetite-containing cells were reported in specific locations in the upper beak.<ref name="Fleissner2003" /> However, the cells proved to be immune system macrophages, not neurons able to detect magnetic fields.<ref name="Treiber2012" /><ref name="Engels 20180124">Template:Cite journal</ref> Second, pigeon magnetic field detection is impaired by sectioning the trigeminal nerve and by application of lidocaine, an anaesthetic, to the olfactory mucosa.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> However, lidocaine treatment might lead to unspecific effects and not represent a direct interference with potential magnetoreceptors.<ref name="Engels 20180124" /> As a result, an involvement of the trigeminal system is still debated. In the search for magnetite receptors, a large iron-containing organelle (the cuticulosome) of unknown function was found in the inner ear of pigeons.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> Areas of the pigeon brain that respond with increased activity to magnetic fields are the posterior vestibular nuclei, dorsal thalamus, hippocampus, and visual hyperpallium.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
Domestic hens have iron mineral deposits in the sensory dendrites in the upper beak and are capable of magnetoreception.<ref name="Falkenberg2010" /><ref name="Wiltschko et al., 2007">Template:Cite journal</ref> Beak trimming causes loss of the magnetic sense.<ref name="Freire et al., 2011">Template:Cite journal</ref>
In mammalsEdit
Some mammals are capable of magnetoreception. When woodmice are removed from their home area and deprived of visual and olfactory cues, they orient towards their homes until an inverted magnetic field is applied to their cage.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> When the same mice are allowed access to visual cues, they are able to orient themselves towards home despite the presence of inverted magnetic fields. This indicates that woodmice use magnetic fields to orient themselves when no other cues are available. The magnetic sense of woodmice is likely based on a radical-pair mechanism.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
The Zambian mole-rat, a subterranean mammal, uses magnetic fields to aid in nest orientation.<ref name="Marhold Wiltschko Burda">Template:Cite journal</ref> In contrast to woodmice, Zambian mole-rats do not rely on radical-pair based magnetoreception, perhaps due to their subterranean lifestyle. Experimental exposure to magnetic fields leads to an increase in neural activity within the superior colliculus, as measured by immediate gene expression. The activity level of neurons within two levels of the superior colliculus, the outer sublayer of the intermediate gray layer and the deep gray layer, were elevated in a non-specific manner when exposed to various magnetic fields. However, within the inner sublayer of the intermediate gray layer (InGi) there were two or three clusters of cells that respond in a more specific manner. The more time the mole rats were exposed to a magnetic field, the greater the immediate early gene expression within the InGi.<ref name="Nemec 2001">Template:Cite journal</ref>
Magnetic fields appear to play a role in bat orientation. They use echolocation to orient themselves over short distances, typically ranging from a few centimetres up to 50 metres.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> When non-migratory big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus) are taken from their home roosts and exposed to magnetic fields rotated 90 degrees from magnetic north, they become disoriented; it is unclear whether they use the magnetic sense as a map, a compass, or a compass calibrator.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> Another bat species, the greater mouse-eared bat (Myotis myotis), appears to use the Earth's magnetic field in its home range as a compass, but needs to calibrate this at sunset or dusk.<ref name="Holland Borissov Siemers 2010">Template:Cite journal</ref> In migratory soprano pipistrelles (Pipistrellus pygmaeus), experiments using mirrors and Helmholtz coils show that they calibrate the magnetic field using the position of the solar disk at sunset.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref><ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>
Red foxes (Vulpes vulpes) may be influenced by the Earth's magnetic field when predating small rodents like mice and voles. They attack these prey using a specific high-jump, preferring a north-eastern compass direction. Successful attacks are tightly clustered to the north.<ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref>
There is not yet a consensus on whether humans can sense magnetic fields or not, but it is being studied and some researchers have found evidence suggesting it.<ref name="Wang Hilburn Wu 2019">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="Chae Human magnetic sense">Template:Cite journal</ref> The ethmoid bone in the nose contains magnetic materials.<ref name="Carrubba Frilot">Template:Cite journal</ref><ref name="Chae Human magnetic sense"/> Magnetosensitive cryptochrome 2 (cry2) is present in the human retina.<ref name="Foley 2011"/> Human alpha brain waves are affected by magnetic fields, but it is not known whether behaviour is affected.<ref name="Wang Hilburn Wu 2019"/><ref name="Foley 2011">Template:Cite journal</ref>