Template:Short description Template:Multiple issues {{#invoke:Infobox|infobox}}Template:Template otherTemplate:Main other

Mam is a Mayan language spoken by about half a million Mam people in the Guatemalan departments of Quetzaltenango, Huehuetenango, San Marcos, and Retalhuleu, and the Mexican states of Campeche and Chiapas. Thousands more make up a Mam diaspora throughout the United States and Mexico, with notable populations living in Oakland, California<ref>Template:Cite news</ref><ref>{{#invoke:citation/CS1|citation |CitationClass=web }}</ref> and Washington, D.C. The most extensive Mam grammar is Nora C. England's A grammar of Mam, a Mayan language (1983), which is based on the San Ildefonso Ixtahuacán dialect of Huehuetenango Department.

ClassificationEdit

Template:See also Mam is closely related to the Tektitek language, and the two languages together form the Mamean sub-branch of the Mayan language family. Along with the Ixilan languages, Awakatek and Ixil, these make up the Greater Mamean sub-branch, one of the two branches of the Eastern Mayan languages (the other being the Greater Quichean sub-branch, which consists of 10 Mayan languages, including Kʼicheʼ).

DialectsEdit

Because contact between members of different Mam communities is somewhat limited, the language varies considerably even from village to village. Nevertheless, mutual intelligibility, though difficult, is possible through practice.Template:Sfn

Mam varieties within Mexico and Guatemala are divided into five dialect groups:<ref>Template:Cite book</ref>

|CitationClass=web }}</ref> Northern Mam is the least conservative group according to Terrence Kaufman.Template:Sfn

|CitationClass=web }}</ref>

|CitationClass=web }}</ref> The Tektitek language may be mutually intelligible with Western Mam dialects.

|CitationClass=web }}</ref>

In addition to these, the dialects of Chiapas, Mexico are characterized by significant grammatical as well as lexical differences from the Guatemalan varieties.<ref name=":1">Template:Cite book</ref>

DistributionEdit

Mam is spoken in 64 communities in four Guatemalan departments<ref>Template:Cite book</ref> and numerous communities in Campeche and Chiapas, Mexico.<ref name=":1" /> Neighboring languages include Jakaltek and Qʼanjobʼal to the north, Tektitek and Qato'k to the west, and Ixil, Awakatek, Sipacapense, and Kʼicheʼ to the east.

Template:Col-begin Template:Col-3 Quetzaltenango Department

Huehuetenango Department

Template:Col-2 San Marcos Department

Retalhuleu Department

Template:Col-3 Chiapas

Campeche

PhonologyEdit

StressEdit

Mam has weight sensitive stress assignment.Template:Sfn Primary stress falls on the long vowel in a word if there is one, e.g. {{#invoke:Lang|lang}} 'work'. Words without a long vowel assign primary stress to the vowel preceding the last glottal stop, e.g. {{#invoke:Lang|lang}} 'dipper'. Words without a long vowel or a glottal stop assign stress to the vowel preceding the last consonant in the root, e.g. {{#invoke:Lang|lang}} 'raccoon'. Stress is not assigned to suffixes or enclitics that do not have long vowels or a glottal stop.

VowelsEdit

Mam has 10 vowels, 5 short and 5 long:Template:Sfn

Front Central Back
Close Long Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme
Short Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme
Mid Long Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme
Short Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme
Open Long Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme
Short Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme
  • A mid-central vowel Template:IPAblink is an allophone of a short unstressed vowel that can occur in the syllable following a stressed long vowel.

ConsonantsEdit

Mam has 27 consonants, including the glottal stop:Template:Sfn

Bilabial Alveolar Postalveolar Retroflex Palatal Velar Uvular Glottal
Plain Palatalized
Nasal Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme (Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme)
Plosive Plain Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme
Ejective Template:IPAlink~Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme
Implosive Template:IPAlink~Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme
Affricate Plain Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme
Ejective Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme
Fricative Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme
Flap Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme
Approximant Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme Template:IPAlink Template:Grapheme
  • Stop sounds /p, t, t͡s, t͡ʃ, t͡ʂ, k, q/ are released with aspiration [Cʰ] in word-final position.
  • Todos Santos Mam has an extended amount of affricate consonants being apical palato-alveolar {{#invoke:IPA|main}}.<ref>Template:Cite book</ref>

Syllable structureEdit

Most roots take the morphological shape CVC.Template:Sfn The only possible root final consonant cluster is -nC. Syllables can have up to four consonants in a cluster in any position. Most consonant clusters are the result of vowel dropping and morpheme addition.Template:Sfn

MorphologyEdit

Mam has two sets of agreement markers, known to Mayanists as Set A and Set B markers, which can appear on both nouns and verbs. Mam uses Set A (ergative) markers on nouns to mark possessor agreement and on verbs to cross-reference the transitive subject. Mam uses Set B (absolutive) markers on transitive verbs to cross-reference the object and on intransitive verbs to cross-reference the subject. Below is a table of Set A (ergative) and Set B (absolutive) prefixes from England.Template:Sfn

Mam Set A and Set B Pronominal Markers
Person Set A Set B Enclitics
1s n- ~ w- chin- -a ~ -ya
2s t- Ø ~ tz- ~ tzʼ- ~ k- -a ~ -ya
3s t- Ø ~ tz- ~ tzʼ- ~ k-
1p (excl.) q- qo- -a ~ -ya
1p (incl.) q- qo-
2p ky- chi- -a ~ -ya
3p ky- chi-

Phonologically conditioned allomorphs are as follows.

  • n- ~ w-
    • n- /__C
    • w- /__V
  • Ø ~ tz- ~ tzʼ- ~ k-
    • k- /potential
    • tzʼ- /__V initial root, non-potential
    • tz- /__uul 'arrive here', iky' 'pass by', non-potential
    • Ø- /__C, non-potential
  • -a ~ -ya
    • -ya /V__ ; In the first person in post-vowel environments, -ya varies freely with -kyʼa and -y'.
    • -a /C__

Some paradigmatic examples from England (1983) are given below. Note that "Ø-" designates a null prefix. Additionally, ma is an aspectual word meaning 'recent past'. Template:Columns-start

Set A markers + NOUN
jaa 'house'
n-jaa-ya 'my house'
t-jaa-ya 'your house'
t-jaa 'his/her house'
q-jaa-ya 'our (not your) house'
q-jaa 'our (everyone's) house'
ky-jaa-ya 'you (pl)'s house'
ky-jaa 'their house'

Template:Column

Set B markers + VERB
bʼeet- to walk
ma chin bʼeet-a 'I walked.'
ma Ø-bʼeet-a 'You walked.'
ma Ø-bʼeet 'He/she walked.'
ma qo bʼeet-a 'We (not you) walked.'
ma qo bʼeet 'We all walked'
ma chi bʼeet-a 'You all walked.'
ma chi bʼeet 'They walked.'

Template:Columns-end

The Mam verb complexEdit

Verbs in Mam can include inflection for person, aspect and mode, as well as auxiliaries in the form of directionals.Template:Sfn The verb complex has distinct forms for transitive and intransitive verb stems depending in part on whether the complex cross-references one or two arguments. The lexical status of the verb complex is ambiguous.Template:Sfn The inflections with vowels are phonologically independent (indicated by spaces). Transitive verb complexes with directionals have a dependent suffix. Two of England's examples of intransitive and transitive verb complexes are shown below.

Intransitive verb complex with directionalTemplate:Sfn Template:Interlinear

Transitive verb complex with directionalTemplate:Sfn Template:Interlinear

Mam extends the Set A (ergative) person markers in the context of focused adverbials and certain subordinate clauses.Template:Sfn In these contexts, the Set A markers cross-reference the subject of intransitive verbs and both the subject and object of transitive verbs. The following examples show the extended ergative marker /t-/ in bold.

Intransitive verb complex with extended ergative markingTemplate:Sfn Template:Interlinear

Transitive verb complex with extended ergative markingTemplate:Sfn Template:Interlinear

<section begin="list-of-glossing-abbreviations"/>

REC:recent past POT:potential aspect ABS:absolutive agreement (Set B) ERG:ergative agreement (Set A) DEP:dependent suffix DIR:directional ENC:person enclitic REL:relational noun PAT:patient

<section end="list-of-glossing-abbreviations"/>

Verb morphemesEdit

Template:Col-begin Template:Col-2 Transitive verbal affixes

  • -bʼaa 'transitivizer' (vowel length can also be used)
  • -laa 'applicative'
  • -wa 'applicative'
  • -bʼV 'causative'
  • -chV 'causative' (variants: -chaa, -chii, -chuu)
  • -kʼuu 'causative'
  • -lV 'causative'
  • -mV 'causative'
  • -nV 'causative'
  • -pV 'causative'
  • -qʼV 'causative'
  • -saa 'causative'
  • -tzii 'causative'
  • -tzʼV 'causative'
  • -txʼii 'causative'
  • -wV 'causative'
  • -najee' 'repetitive'
  • -'kJ 'processive'
  • -'tz 'processive imperative'

Intransitive verbal affixes

  • -n 'antipassive'
  • -Vn 'affect'
  • -ax 'versive'
  • -ee' 'versive'
  • -eet 'passive'
  • -j 'passive'
  • -njtz 'passive'
  • -bʼaj 'processive passive'
  • -bʼa 'intransitivizer'
  • -ch 'intransitivizer'
  • -chaj 'intransitivizer'
  • -paj 'intransitivizer'
  • -t 'intransitivizer'
  • -tzʼaj 'intransitivizer'
  • -tzʼaq 'intransitivizer'
  • -' ... -al 'specific termination'

Template:Col-2 Other verbal affixes

  • -l 'infinitive'

Aspects

Mam verbs have 6 aspects that are prefixed to the verb root.Template:Sfn

  • ma 'recent past'
  • o 'past'
  • ok 'potential' (not obligatory)
  • n- 'progressive'
  • x- 'recent past dependent' (used in subordinate clauses)
  • Ø- 'past dependent' (used in subordinate clauses)

Modes

  • Potential transitive: -a'
  • Potential intransitive: -l
  • Imperative: -m (-n before directionals)

Directionals

Directionals are auxiliary elements in verb phrases. They are derived from intransitive verbs.

  • xi 'away from'
  • tzaj 'toward'
  • ul 'there to here'
  • pon 'here to there'
  • kubʼ 'down'
  • jaw 'up'
  • el 'out'
  • ok 'in'
  • kyaj 'remaining'
  • aj 'returning from here'
  • ikyʼ 'passing'
  • bʼaj 'complete'

Template:Col-end

PronounsEdit

Mam has no independent pronouns.Template:Sfn Rather, pronouns in Mam always exist as bound morphemes.

NounsEdit

The Mam language displays inalienable possession. Certain Mam nouns cannot be possessed, such as kya'j 'sky' and che'w 'star'.Template:Sfn On the other hand, some Mam nouns are always possessed, such as t-lokʼ 'its root' and t-bʼaqʼ 'its seed'.

Noun phrase structure can be summarized into the following template.Template:Sfn

Demonstrative Number Measure Plural Possessive affixes NOUN
ROOT
Possessor Adjective Relative clause

The plural clitic is qa.Template:Cn

Template:Col-begin Template:Col-2 Noun affixes

  • aj- 'agent'
  • aj- 'native'
  • -l 'agentive'
  • -eenj 'patient'
  • -bʼil 'instrumental'
  • -bʼeen 'resultant locative'
  • -bʼan 'reminder'
  • -al 'abstract noun'
  • -abʼiil 'abstract noun'
  • -leen 'abstract noun'
  • -le'n 'abstract noun'
  • -an 'ordinal'
  • -bʼji'bʼil 'nominalizer'
  • -bʼal 'nominalizer'
  • -bʼatz 'nominalizer'
  • -l 'nominalizer'
  • -tl 'nominalizer'
  • -tz 'nominalizer'

Relational noun affixes

  • -u'n 'agent, instrument, causative'
  • -ee 'dative, possessive, patient, benefactive'
  • -i'j 'patient, thematic'
  • -uukʼal 'instrument, comitative'
  • -iibʼaj 'reflexive'

Locative affixes

  • -bʼutxʼ 'at the corner'
  • -i'jla 'around'
  • -iibʼaj 'over'
  • -jaqʼ 'under'
  • -txa'n 'at the edge of'
  • -txlaj 'beside'
  • -tzii' 'at the entrance of'
  • -uj 'in'
  • -witz 'on top of'
  • -wi' 'on, at the tip of'

Template:Col-2 Classifiers

  • jal 'non-human'
  • nu'xh 'baby'
  • xhlaaqʼ 'child'
  • bʼixh 'person of the same status (intimate)'
  • qʼa 'young man'
  • txin 'young woman'
  • ma 'man'
  • xu'j 'woman'
  • swe'j 'old man'
  • xhyaa' 'old woman'
  • xnuq 'old man (respectful)'
  • xuj 'old man (respectful)'

Measure words

Measure words quantify mass nouns.

  • baas 'glassful' (< Spanish vaso)
  • ma'l 'shot of liquor'
  • laq 'plateful'
  • pixh 'piece'
  • txut 'drop'
  • ba'uj 'a lot'

Template:Col-end

NumeralsEdit

San Ildefonso Ixtahuacán Mam numbers are as follows.Template:Sfn Numbers above twenty are rarely used in Ixtahuacán and are usually only known by elderly speakers. Although the number system would have originally been vigesimal (i.e., base 20), the present-day number system of Ixtahuacán is now decimal.

Numeral Word
1 lang}}
2 lang}}
3 lang}}
4 lang}}
5 lang}}
6 lang}}
7 lang}}
8 lang}}
9 lang}}
10 lang}}
20 lang}}
40 lang}}
60 lang}}
80 lang}}

SyntaxEdit

Mam has both verbal and non-verbal types of sentences. Verbal sentences have verbal predicates, whereas non-verbal sentences have a stative or a locative/existential predicate.Template:Sfn Verbal predicates have an aspect marker, while non-verbal predicates do not have aspect marking. Both verbal and non-verbal predicates occur in sentence-initial position unless a focused or topicalized phrase is present.

Verbal predicatesEdit

Verbal predicates are either transitive or intransitive according to the number of arguments cross-referenced in the verb complex. The number of arguments cross-referenced by the verb complex is not consistent with the transitivity of the verb root or the number of participants in an event. England notes examples of transitive verb roots that only appear in their antipassive or passive forms where they only cross-reference a single participant.

  • Transitive verb root with obligatory antipassive voiceTemplate:Sfn

Template:Interlinear

  • Transitive verb root with obligatory passive voiceTemplate:Sfn

Template:Interlinear

Another possibility is the use of intransitive motion verbs to express transitive events.Template:Sfn

  • Intransitive motion verbs expressing transitive eventsTemplate:Sfn

Template:Interlinear

Template:Interlinear

The basic word order in verbal sentences with two nominal arguments is VSO.Template:Sfn Other word orders are not acceptable.

Template:Interlinear

If only one argument appears in a transitive sentence and the argument is compatible with either person marker on the verb, it has a patient interpretation.Template:Sfn

Template:Interlinear

Mam speakers use a higher proportion of intransitive sentences than speakers of other Mayan languages. England and Martin (2003) found a low frequency of transitive sentences in Mam texts.Template:Full citation needed Pye (2017) found a low use of overt subjects in transitive sentences in adults speaking to children. One adult produced overt subjects in 6% of transitive sentences. The same adult produced overt subjects in 41% of intransitive sentences and produced overt objects in 49% of transitive sentences.Template:Sfn

Non-verbal predicatesEdit

Mam adds Set B person markers to nouns and adjectives to form non-verbal predicates. The following Set B person markers are used for non-verbal predicates (i.e., nouns, adjectives). Also, in statives, aa can be omitted when the rest of the stative is a non-enclitic (in other words, a separate, independent word).

Mam Set B Pronominal Markers
(non-verbal predicates)
Person Stative<ref>Means 'This is X.'</ref> Locative / Existental<ref>Means 'X is in a place.'</ref>
1s (aa) qiin-a (a)t-iin-a
2s aa-ya (a)t-(aʼ-y)a
3s aa (a)t-(aʼ)
1p (excl.) (aa) qoʼ-ya (a)t-oʼ-ya
1p (incl.) (aa) qoʼ (a)t-oʼ
2p aa-qa-ya (a)t-eʼ-ya
3p aa-qa (a)t-eʼ

Paradigmatic examples of non-verbal predicates from England (1983) are given below.Template:Sfn Template:Columns-start

NOUN + Set B markers
xjaal person
xjaal qiin-a 'I am a person.'
xjaal-a 'You are a person.'
xjaal 'He/she is a person.'
xjaal qoʼ-ya 'We (excl.) are persons.'
xjaal qo- 'We (incl.) are persons.'
xjaal qa-ya 'You all are persons.'
xjaal qa 'They are persons.'

Template:Column

ADJECTIVE + Set B markers
sikynaj tired
sikynaj qiin-a 'I am tired.'
sikynaj-a 'You are tired.'
sikynaj 'He/she is tired.'
sikynaj qoʼ-ya 'We (excl.) are tired.'
sikynaj qoʼ 'We (incl.) are tired.'
sikynaj qa-ya 'You all are tired.'
sikynaj qa 'They are tired.'

Template:Columns-end

<section begin="list-of-glossing-abbreviations"/>

REC:recent past AP:antipassive suffix PAS:passive suffix POT:potential aspect ABS:absolutive agreement (Set B) ERG:ergative agreement (Set A) DEP:dependent suffix DIR:directional ENC:person enclitic INTENS:intensive REL:relational noun PAT:patient TV:transitive verb IMP:imperative CL:noun classifier

<section end="list-of-glossing-abbreviations"/>

Child LanguageEdit

An overview of child language acquisition in Mam can be found in Pye (2017). Child language data for Mam challenge many theories of language acquisition and demonstrate the need for more extensive documentation of native American languages.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref>

Children acquiring Mam produce a higher proportion of verbs than children acquiring K’iche’, but a lower proportion of verbs compared to children acquiring Wastek and Chol. They produce a higher proportion of intransitive verbs relative to transitive verbs than children acquiring other Mayan languages (Pye, Pfeiler and Mateo Pedro 2017:22).Template:Clarification needed Their high proportion of relational noun production is tied to their frequent use of intransitive verbs.Template:Sfn

The following examples illustrate the children's use of intransitive verbs to express events with two participants. Ages are shown as (years;months.days). WEN (2;0.2) used the intransitive verb {{#invoke:Lang|lang}} ("go_down") in reference to an event of picking coffee. She used the relational noun phrase {{#invoke:Lang|lang}} to express the agent in an oblique phrase. CRU (2;5.12) used the intransitive verb {{#invoke:Lang|lang}} ("go_out") in reference to an event of taking out an object. She used the relational noun phrase {{#invoke:Lang|lang}} to express the agent. JOS (2;6.17) used the intransitive verb {{#invoke:Lang|lang}} ("finish") in reference to finishing a drink. He used the possessive prefix on the noun {{#invoke:Lang|lang}} ("drink")to express the agent. The examples overturn the hypothesis that children tie their use of transitive verbs to object manipulation events.

  • WEN (2;0.2)

Template:Fs interlinear

  • CRU (2;5.12)

Template:Fs interlinear

  • JOS (2;6.17)

Template:Fs interlinear

Two-year-old Mam children produce the consonants {{#invoke:IPA|main}}. They produce Template:IPAblink in place of glottalized stops, Template:IPAblink in place of Template:IPAslink, Template:IPAblink in place of Template:IPAslink and Template:IPAslink, Template:IPAblink in place of Template:IPAslink and Template:IPAslink, Template:IPAblink in place of Template:IPAslink, and Template:IPAblink in place of Template:IPAslink. Mam children begin producing ejective consonants after they are three and a half years old.<ref>Template:Cite journal</ref> The early production of /t͡ʃ/ and /l/ in Mam, as well as the late production of /s/, overturns predictions that all children have similar phonologies due to articulatory development.Template:Cn

Template:Cn span

The following examples illustrate WEN’s verb complex production.Template:Sfn In (1), WEN produced the vowel /a/ from the verb root {{#invoke:Lang|lang}} ("give"), the imperative suffix -n, and the directional suffix -tz as /xh/. (Many directionals have contracted forms as suffixes.). WEN omitted the person enclitic -a. In (2), WEN produced the progressive prefix n-, the vowel /e/ from the verb root {{#invoke:Lang|lang}} ("go out"), a spurious /n/, and the directional suffix -tz as /ch/. The intransitive verb {{#invoke:Lang|lang}} belongs to the class of motion verbs that take directional suffixes. Intransitive verbs outside of the class of motion verbs do not take directional suffixes except in imperative contexts. The verb {{#invoke:Lang|lang}} contracts with the directional suffix -tz to produce the stem {{#invoke:Lang|lang}} ("go out to") in adult speech. WEN’s omission of the person enclitic and production of a spurious consonant overturn the hypothesis that children produce forms that are frequent in adult speech.

  • WEN (1;9.2)

Template:Fs interlinear

  • WEN (1;8.21)

Template:Fs interlinear

The children’s production of the directional suffixes demonstrates their early recognition of the distinction between intransitive and transitive verbs in Mam. This distinction is a core feature of Mam grammar, and underpins the ergative morphology on the verbs and nouns. The semantic diversity of the verbs and positionals overturns the hypothesis that children use prototypical activity scenes as a basis for constructing grammatical categories. The children’s grammatical acumen is best seen in their use of the ergative and absolutive agreement markers on verbs. The children produced the prevocalic allomorphs of the ergative markers in nearly all of their obligatory contexts. They produced the preconsonantal allomorphs of the ergative markers in 20% of their obligatory contexts.Template:Sfn

Two-year-old Mam children display a remarkable awareness of the contexts for extending the use of ergative markers to cross-reference the subject of intransitive verbs. Outside of these contexts, they consistently produced absolutive person markers on intransitive verbs. Three Mam children produced ergative person markers on intransitive verbs in half of the obligatory contexts for extended ergativity.Template:Sfn The children’s awareness of the contexts for extended ergative use is all the more remarkable because the contexts are tied to clauses in dependent contexts in which aspect is not overtly marked.Template:Sfn The following example shows JOS’s use of extended ergative marking (in bold) on the intransitive verb {{#invoke:Lang|lang}} ("go_in") in a purpose clause headed by the adverb {{#invoke:Lang|lang}} ("so that"). The children’s production of ergative markers on intransitive verbs in dependent contexts overturns the theory that children link ergative markers to the subjects of transitive verbs in all contexts.

  • JOS (2;6.14)

Template:Fs interlinear

Mam two-year-olds produce sentences with a predicate-initial word order. The children, like adults, rarely produce the subject argument in transitive sentences. The Mam children show an ergative pattern of argument production that similar to the adult pattern.Template:Sfn Template:Cn span

Template:Cn span

<section begin="list-of-glossing-abbreviations"/>

REC:recent past AP:antipassive suffix PAS:passive suffix POT:potential aspect ABS:absolutive agreement (Set B) ERG:ergative agreement (Set A) DEP:dependent suffix DIR:directional ENC:person enclitic INTENS:intensive REL:relational noun PAT:patient TV:transitive verb IMP:imperative CL:noun classifier

<section end="list-of-glossing-abbreviations"/>

ReferencesEdit

NotesEdit

Template:Reflist

BibliographyEdit

Further readingEdit

External linksEdit

Template:Incubator

Template:Mayan languages Template:Languages of Guatemala Template:Languages of Mexico

Template:Authority control